Huberman Lab: Dr. Immordino-Yang: How Emotions & Social Factors Impact Learning

Scicomm Media Scicomm Media 6/5/23 - Episode Page - 2h 42m - PDF Transcript

Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast where we discuss science and science-based tools

for everyday life.

I'm Andrew Huberman and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology at Stanford

School of Medicine.

Today my guest is Dr. Mary Helen Immordino-Yang.

Dr. Immordino-Yang is a professor of education, psychology and neuroscience at the University

of Southern California.

Her laboratory focuses on emotions and the role of emotions in learning as well as how

social interactions impact how we learn.

Today's discussion is one that I found absolutely fascinating because it will reveal to you,

in fact to all of us, how our temperament, that is our emotionality, combined with our

home environment and the school environments that we were raised in shape what we know

about the world and our concepts of self.

In thinking about that we also discuss the education system and how different aspects

of rules and how we are told to behave and what actually constitutes good behavior or

bad behavior shape how we learn information and develop a sense of meaning in life.

If any of that sounds abstract I promise you that today's discussion is incredibly practical.

You will learn, for instance, how different styles of learning are going to favor different

people from children into adulthood and how we ought to think about learning in terms

of our emotional systems being our guide for what we learn and the information that

we retain and how we apply that information throughout life.

For those of you that are parents or who are thinking of becoming parents or who were once

children, so I believe that encompasses everybody out there, today's discussion will arm you

with an intellectual understanding of psychology and neuroscience as it relates to learning

but also practical tools that you can apply in order to be able to learn more effectively.

What I like so much about Dr. Immordino Yang's research and the discussion today is that

she frames up beautifully how those who best learn from traditional forms of classroom learning

as well as those who learn from non-traditional forms of learning either in or out of the

classroom can best use that understanding of self in order to learn in the way that is

best for them.

Before we begin I'd like to emphasize that this podcast is separate from my teaching

and research roles at Stanford.

It is however part of my desire and effort to bring zero cost to consumer information

about science and science related tools to the general public.

In keeping with that theme I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast.

Our first sponsor is AteSleep.

AteSleep makes smart mattress covers with cooling, heating, and sleep tracking capacity.

I've talked many times before on this podcast about the fact that sleep is the foundation

of mental health, physical health, and performance of all kinds.

One of the key things to getting a great night's sleep is the temperature of your sleeping environment

and that's because your core body temperature actually has to drop by about one to three

degrees in order for you to get into and stay deeply asleep and conversely your core body

temperature increases by about one to three degrees in order for you to wake up and feel

refreshed.

With AteSleep you can control the temperature of your sleeping environment very easily because

of the way that the mattress cover communicates with an app where you can dial in the temperature

of your sleeping environment at the beginning, middle, and end of your night as you arrive

toward morning.

Moving on an AteSleep mattress cover has greatly enhanced the quality of my sleep.

I know that because it also includes a sleep tracker, which will tell you how much slow

wave sleep and rapid eye movement sleep you're getting and it gives you a sleep score.

If you'd like to try AteSleep, go to AteSleep.com slash Huberman and get up to $150 off.

AteSleep currently ships in the USA, Canada, UK, select countries in the EU, and Australia.

Again, that's AteSleep.com slash Huberman.

Today's episode is also brought to us by HVMN Ketone IQ.

Ketone IQ is a ketone supplement that increases blood ketones.

I know most people are familiar with or at least have heard of the so-called ketogenic

diet.

It's used for weight loss.

It's used to control epilepsy.

It's used for mental health reasons.

However, most people, including myself, do not follow a ketogenic diet.

Nonetheless, increasing your blood ketones can improve the function of your brain and

the function of your body.

And that's because ketones are preferred use of fuel for the brain and body.

So even though I follow an omnivore diet, that is, I'm not in a ketogenic state, I

use ketone IQ to increase my blood ketones prior to doing preparation for podcasts or

writing grants or doing research, as well as prior to workouts, especially if I want

to work out fasted, I'll take some ketone IQ to increase my blood ketones, which gives

me a lot of energy during workouts or during bouts of cognitive work, even if I haven't

eaten in the preceding hours, it really increases my focus and my energy levels.

If you'd like to try ketone IQ, you can go to HVMN.com slash Huberman to save 20% off.

Today's episode is also brought to us by Roka.

Roka makes eyeglasses and sunglasses that are the absolute highest quality.

The company was founded by two All-American swimmers from Stanford, and everything about

Roka eyeglasses and sunglasses were designed with performance in mind.

I've spent a lifetime working on the biology, the visual system, and I can tell you that

your visual system has to contend with an enormous number of different challenges in

order for you to be able to see clearly.

Roka understands those challenges and has designed their eyeglasses and sunglasses accordingly

so that you always see with perfect clarity.

Their eyeglasses and sunglasses were initially designed for sports performance, and as a

consequence, they are very lightweight, which is great.

They also won't slip off your face if you get sweaty.

However, even though they were designed for sports performance, they now also include

a lot of styles that are designed to be worn to work, out to dinner, essentially recreationally

so that you could wear anywhere.

If you'd like to try Roka eyeglasses or sunglasses, go to Roka.

That's R-O-K-A.com and enter the code Huberman to save 20% off your order.

Again, that's Roka R-O-K-A.com and enter the code Huberman at checkout.

And now for my discussion with Dr. Mary Helen Immordino Yang.

Dr. Immordino Yang.

Good to be here.

Great to have you.

I'd like to start off talking about something that to me seems a little bit high level,

but I think is the perfect jumping off point.

I've heard you talk before about inspiration and awe.

And as somebody who's interested in the brain and as somebody who's interested in the role

of emotions and learning and life experience, inspiration and awe seem to me rather high

level emotional experiences compared to say fear or happiness.

And yet inspiration and awe just seems so fundamental to how we learn and navigate life.

And before we started recording, we were talking about David Goggins of all people and we'll

get back to that.

But if you could just share with us, what is the role of inspiration and awe and story

in how we learn and experience life starting at a young age and then maybe we can transition

to older ages?

Yeah, I mean, I think what you've noticed is actually fundamental to the conundrum of

being a human is that our most high level complex brain states, mind states are also

fundamentally hooking themselves into the most basic biological machinery that literally

we share with alligators that keeps us alive.

And that is both the power and the potential of being a human and the danger of it.

So our beliefs, our experiences, our interpretations of the meaning of things, which that's where

the story comes in, the stories that we conjure about collectively with other people, culturally

in spaces inside our own selves also, those stories become kind of the through line that

organizes the way in which we construct our own experience consciousness, even I would

say.

So when we hook into those very basic survival systems by recruiting them into these narratives

about the nature of reality, the power of the meaning we make, what happens is we get this

amazingly both fundamental and high level state simultaneously where we feel expansive,

we feel like it's all so incredibly beautiful.

And we are, I would argue, actually ramping into or catching into the very basic survival

mechanisms that make us conscious, that make us alive.

And that's in essence the power of being a human.

That's the power of our intelligence at this late stage in our evolution.

So when I was a kid, I loved stories of all kinds.

I think like most kids, I loved my curious George books.

I'm told I like the Babar books, but then quickly didn't like the Babar books.

I liked the book where the red fern grows.

I liked books and stories about it generally was boys for me, for whatever reason, that

had some idea in mind or some ongoing challenge.

And that played out over time and the character evolves across the story.

And of course, many, many, many excellent stories have all those features.

I can recall specific passages in those books to this day that made me feel something in

my body.

I actually am very familiar with the sensation of having chills go up my spine as opposed

to down my spine.

Early on, I realized, oh, there's sort of a difference.

Sometimes it travels up my spine, sometimes I haven't distinguished what that orients

me to or away from.

But it's a very salient memory and experience for me to this day.

So much so that as I'm describing the book where the red fern grows right now, I can

kind of feel it starting, yeah.

I've heard you say before, and I love this quote, and I want to make sure that you get

attribution for this, not me, that we basically have a brain to control our body.

What is the role of the brain in controlling the body?

Do you think that there are an infinite number of ways in which our brain does that?

Or are we really talking about a language between brain and body of tingles on the back

of our neck that go up, tingles on the back of our neck that go down, stomach feeling

kind of tight and making us cringe away or kind of warm and wanting to approach?

In other words, do you think that the conversation between the brain and body is primitive, sophisticated?

How nuanced is it?

Because language is very nuanced.

You could probably come up with 50 words just in English for the state of being happy.

But the feeling of being happy, I experience along a continuum of a little bit happy to

elated, but it's kind of one thing, really.

So if you would, could you comment on this notion of the brain being the organ that's

responsible for controlling the body and what that dialogue is like?

What the syllables and consonants of it are like?

It's not at the level of biology, but at the level of psychology and how we subjectively

experience that.

Sure.

So the first thing I'll say is that I learned that idea from working with Antonia DiMazio.

So he was my postdoctoral mentor and he taught me first that this notion that it's the feeling

of the body, it's an organism's ability to represent or map the state of the interior

and exterior of the body that becomes the substrate for consciousness and for the mind.

So I would just want to give him credit because I didn't think of that first.

But the work that I've been doing is an elaboration of that.

It's basically addressing exactly the question that you're asking, which is how is it that

we construct a narrative, construct a conscious feeling, which that word I take from Antonia

and Hannah, right, DiMazio?

How is it that we construct a feeling and sort of narratize that feeling, elaborate

that feeling into something that feels like a narrative, that feels like a belief state

or an emotion state or an experience, I mean, that in a very verb-like way?

And what is the role of embodiment in that?

What is the role of the brain in that?

And what also is the role of the culture and the cultural context and other people in that?

What we're really learning across the sciences right now is just how incredibly social and

interdependent our species is.

I mean, our biology is inherently a social one.

We are directly dependent on other people for the formulation of our own sense of self,

and we interact with one another and construct and co-construct a sense of self and a sense

of meaning via those cultural spaces and those sort of nuanced ways of accommodating

each other mentally and physically that lead to the feeling of us.

So back to your original question, there's a lot we don't know there, but I think what's

very clear is that the kind of background sense of the body, the mapping and the regulation

of the body is a basic substrate, a kind of trampling for the mind.

And so we are managing our survival.

We now have lots of evidence from across many kinds of science about the interdependence

of our stress and social relationships and our immunity and our ability to digest food.

And it's even now very clear that it's not even just us.

There's a whole microbiome and all kinds of other organisms that are assisting us in

that and that are collaborating with us in that.

And then the brain is a specialized organ of the body.

In fact, it's not a separate thing.

It's an outgrowth or an elaboration of that process.

It's a specialization of that process, a localization of it, in a way that provides enough processing

power to be able to really construct all kinds of feelings and mental states and beliefs

and imaginings out of basically just the feeling of being here.

And then the amazing part is that our brain is also imposing those back down onto our

body, so the way in which our body reacts and is modulated in response to mental states

is also very real.

So we have a kind of like a dynamic conversation happening that's happening in very raw and

direct ways, neurochemically and others, and also in broader, longer term, slower fluctuating

patterns around other kinds of hormonal changes and things like that.

So along multiple timescales simultaneously, we have a kind of whole, a humanistic whole

of brain and body and mind that are kind of co-conjuring one another in real time.

And that leads to all kinds of dynamic possibility spaces for how we are and how we feel as we

grow through time.

And I think as humans, the legacy of our intelligence is to tap into those possibility spaces and

start to construct them into meaningful sort of chains of ideas, chains of experiences

over time that we call story.

And that I think is what you were tapping into as a little boy.

You were hungry for fodder, for a kind of structure for those feelings that you could

start to help them evolve from one into the other and chain them together in ways that

produce meaning.

Yeah, I'm fascinated by the idea that early in life, we experience some interaction with

the world.

It could be with other people, it could be with an object in the world, and it makes

us feel something powerful.

And that lays a template of recognition, meaning that later in life and perhaps throughout

life, we're always consciously or subconsciously going back to trying to experience that same

kind of awe or inspiration.

Because again, the circumstances almost certainly vary from being a five-year-old to being a

adolescent and into adulthood and into the, I guess, the geriatric years.

I probably used the politically incorrect term, but forgive me, 75 to 125.

And yet the feeling is the same, right?

And so it's as if a word can mean the same thing, but be used 50 different ways, maybe

5,000 different ways.

And in this analogy, I'm saying that the word is the feeling and it's used so many different

ways because occasionally I'll read a scientific manuscript and I'll go, that is so cool.

And it's the same way that I feel when I was nine years old and I spent all my time in

the pet store looking at tropical fish and tropical birds and thinking, oh my God, that

freshwater discus fish is the coolest thing I've ever seen.

And again, I think I must have a strong memory for these kinds of things because I still,

I feel it right now in my body.

So it's as if the same thing maps to so many different circumstances.

So is what we're learning across the lifespan a recognition of feelings in our body as,

ah, this is something I like because of the way it makes my body feel?

Or is it cognitive or both?

From your answer a moment ago, it seems like it's so interconnected and bi-directional

and fast that it's impossible to really say that feelings are in the body or in the brain.

It's really happening simultaneously.

Yeah, it's a dynamic emergent state.

Let me give you an example that I use sometimes to help myself understand the notion.

So my little daughter, Nora, when she was two, two and some months, two and four months

of that, she's a very verbal kid.

And I was sitting in the kitchen one day drinking a cup of tea.

I was sad about something that happened in my life, but I wasn't weeping or anything.

I was just sitting there.

I must have looked kind of lost in my own thoughts.

I was just playing around on the floor.

She came over to me, I'll never forget it, this tiny little person.

She comes over to me and noticed I wasn't really there with her, you know what I mean?

And she, my arm was hanging down.

She picked up my arm and she held it against her face like that.

And she said, I won't say in baby talk because you won't understand, but she said, don't worry,

mama, I'll take care of you.

And I said, yeah.

And I said, oh, Nora, that's so sweet, sweetie, I'll take care of you too.

And she said, and mama, I will, we love you.

I really love you.

And then she said, I mean, I really love y'all, oh, I really love your arm, right?

Fast forward two years later, almost exactly two years, she's four in a couple months.

And she was in bed one night, laying in her bed in the dark, and I walked by and I listened

at the door to see if she was sleeping there.

And I hear this little whisper comes out and she says, mama, I love you more than I'm glad

that there's daytime, right?

That's changed developmentally from her at age two to her at age four, right?

I would argue that the physiological substrate of her attachment to her mother is probably

quite similar.

She had this sort of visceral, automatic, biological, you might say, attachment, connection

to me emotionally that she was trying to leverage in the service of making sense of, you know,

being active in that world and adapting herself to the situation, helping me in the first

case, right?

But what's changed remarkably is not the substrate of that attachment, it's her ability to conceptualize

it, right?

When she's two, her love is experienced as this incredibly concrete, embodied, real

physical thing.

Like, I love you, I mean, I really love the body part, I am currently smooshing against

my face, right?

Whereas two years later, she can conceptualize that love in terms of an idea, which is, you

know, wouldn't it be awful if there was nighttime all the time and there was no sunshine and

daylight and I couldn't go out to play and I couldn't, right?

You're describing my biggest fear at people, listeners of this podcast, I know that I'm

going to go into the grave, hopefully a long time from now, telling people to get morning

sunlight in their eyes.

I know, I still do it to those of you.

But please continue.

No, but that's great.

So she's thinking about how much she is grateful for there to be sunlight.

And in her little mind, she connected that to the feeling of being attached to me and

used one to explain the other, right?

So that both things now have meaning.

And that is the way, that is the way I think that we start to elaborate these very basic

physiological attachment states, aversion states, right?

Mental states of various sorts into mental states, beliefs, poems, you know, love songs,

all the things that she does, right?

Even between age two and age four, that really are mental elaborations, meaning making of

that very physiologically basic sensation.

Does that answer your question?

It answers it incredibly clearly.

So much so that I'd like to continue to build on that example, because I think it's very

relatable for people.

And it's the first time that I've ever heard the embodiment of emotions described in a

developmental framework that truly makes sense.

So thank you.

So the contact with your arm or your arm or both was the life example that she was using

it as a two-year-old that maps to an internal feeling.

And we're going to assume she's not here.

We don't have her in a brain scanner.

We can't ask her.

But we're going to assume that her experience of being put to bed at night and feeling so

much love from and for you, mapped to her then growing understanding of the world around

her and the fact that there's day and night and sunshine.

So as her knowledge base grows, she can add examples to the feeling.

And I'm assuming that doesn't matter how old she is now, but I'm assuming that as a 14-year-old,

the knowledge base is going to be different and is going to map to that feeling again

and again.

The question is, is what we are doing across the lifespan is recognizing sort of, I don't

want to call them primitives, but basic emotional states, which are not infinite, but can be

each one along a continuum.

So a little bit of love, completely in love, along a continuum and everything in between,

a little angry and annoyed to completely furious.

Are we talking about maybe 10 to 30 core emotions that then we are just simply binning our experiences

into and onto and mapping onto and then that's our life story.

And I'm not trying to oversimplify things, but that seems to me like a pretty great way

for a nervous system to navigate a world that is infinitely complex and has a lot

of surprise, both positive and negative, and in which, like every organism, our main goal

is to survive as long as possible and not for everybody, but in many cases to try and

make more of ourselves, I mean, though something to be the basic drive, survive and make more

of oneself.

It seems to be the two basic functions of every of your ideas or more of your work, more of

your art.

Right.

Exactly.

So is that an overly simplistic way to think about it or does it does it work, even if

there's more that needs to be added?

Does that work?

As a 20 year old, I learned things in college and I'm like, this is awesome.

The first time I learned about the hypothalamus, this little marble size structure and the

fact that different neurons sitting right next to each other can put us into a rage

where we'll make us want to mate or we'll make us thirsty or hungry or tired.

I was like, wow, I mean, it just blew me away.

It still blows me away.

But the feeling is the same as looking at the discus fish in Monet's Pet Shop on California

Avenue when I'm nine years old.

So is that the way to think about it?

I think, yes, I think there's an awful lot of basic physiological mechanisms that become

motivational mechanisms in all the senses, adaptive mechanisms that we share with all

life forms, not even just all animals, but all life forms, but they look different and

different life forms for sure because the adaptive functions, the time scales and everything

are different.

If you're a tree, then if you're a fish, then if you're a slime mold or you're me, right?

But I think you're right that what we basically are doing is taking these very primitive physiological

regulatory capacities that are essentially there to keep you alive.

And that's a very dynamic thing to keep you alive.

You have to constantly adjust for the needs of the internal organism, the needs of the

external, the demands of the external environment on that organism and being able to manage

in that space over time is a very complex, dynamic, kind of iterative process.

And we take those processes and we conjure out of them a form of consciousness, an awareness

of those processes that becomes something that feels mentally powerful to us.

And I think one of the ways that we can know that what you're saying is right is that this

was just our first experiment on this, but I think it's really poignant.

We first started to study the ways people would react to social stimuli, right?

To have emotions like compassion or admiration in the MRI scanner by telling people stories

of true people's situations that invoked these emotions in all kinds of piloting.

And then we ask people, how does it make you feel?

And then we can see whether they actually feel that way.

And then we move them into the MRI scanner and ask them again to watch the story and

feel it.

And what we expected, we had some very basic hypotheses that things like watching somebody

else endure physical pain would activate the same systems in your brain that allow you

to feel physical pain and the same with pleasure around admiration for skill, watching somebody

do flips on their bike on a railroad tie or whatever it is, or virtue, watching a civil

rights leader or somebody who does something that's incredibly virtuously powerful, but

not physically skilled.

And we had a real surprise in those findings, which I think really went against the prevailing

notion of how emotion works and which is still something which I wrestle with trying to understand.

So we hypothesized that feeling emotions about very physical direct things and feeling emotions

about drawing them in space, but feeling emotions about complex, elaborated things like compassion

for someone having lost a mouse or something where you don't see any real physical pain,

but you can imagine how they're feeling based on your shared experience of loss, right?

Or admiration for virtue, that those things would build neurobiologically the way that

they build developmentally, the way that they build evolutionarily.

And we did find that to be the case, and many other groups and experiments have found that

too.

And the real surprise to us is that emotions based in pain and emotions based in something

rewarding or pleasurable like virtue, which is really inspiring as people describe it,

we're actually recruiting the same brain systems, including the hypothalamus, right?

And other systems like the anterior insula, which is basically visceral somatomotor cortex.

It's cortex that feels the state of how you're digesting your lunch, whether your heart's

pounding, all these kinds of things, right?

What we found is that these emotions, when they get complex, when they're about stories,

the valence is no longer the defining feature.

The valence doesn't even matter that much.

Instead what matters is, does the emotion pertain to a story that is conjured in our

minds or does it mainly pertain to what you can directly witness by looking at the person?

So they step off a curb, they break their ankle and you go, oh, that looks like it really

hurt, right?

Versus they're eating dinner alone in a restaurant and somebody tells you his spouse died just

a month ago, right?

Where you have to tell yourself an entire story about how he must be feeling in that

situation as compared to just looking at him and seeing the ankle and going, ooh, you know?

And it was that leap, which is really uniquely human, which is fully developed really throughout

a very protracted period, right?

Little children do not fully appreciate those kinds of mental states yet, right?

And in adolescence, kids are all about trying to conjure and simulate these things and they

do it very, you know, they overdo it and they do it in these very sort of awkward ways that

adults recognize as, you know, not likely to correspond fully to reality, right?

Many times.

And then we start to build more and more facility, more and more sort of wisdom around conjuring

the story that makes the most direct parsimonious sense out of the things that you imagine somebody

else may have experienced given the complexities of the context in which they find themselves.

It becomes more and more dynamic, more and more sort of inferential.

And so this also goes back to what you were saying about development.

This is actually how I see development across the lifespan.

My little two-year-old loves the arm, then she loves me as much as something else that

she really appreciates like daylight.

And then she goes on from there and when she's 80, God willing, someday, right, she'll be

making a different kind of story, picking out things that matter in more subtle ways

that other people may not notice because of the historical context, because of her more

lived experience that she brings to that story, right?

So the things that become salient, the things you learn how to notice and build a story

out of are developmental and they're learned across time.

But the basic fundamental processes around the emotions are always driving the need to

make the story.

And so just to come back answering what you said before, I think we have this incredibly

complex dynamic set of basic emotions or whatever you want to call them, physiological states

that we share with other organisms that are basically action programs that teach you run

away from this, move toward that, eat this, don't eat that, right?

But those things in humans and to a lesser degree in other animals become the fodder

for not just action programs in the moment, but ideas that transcend time, ideas that

become the narratives of the stuff of beliefs, of values, of identities.

Those more ethereal, you know, essences of us that are conjured entirely by us in cultural

spaces are fundamentally grounded into our ability to experience the world in a real

physical embodied sense, but elaborated far beyond that.

I'd like to take a quick break and acknowledge one of our sponsors, Athletic Greens.

Athletic Greens now called AG1 is a vitamin mineral probiotic drink that covers all of

your foundational nutritional needs.

I've been taking Athletic Greens since 2012, so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring

the podcast.

The reason I started taking Athletic Greens and the reason I still take Athletic Greens

once or usually twice a day is that it gets to be the probiotics that I need for gut health.

Our gut is very important.

It's populated by gut microbiota that communicate with the brain, the immune system, and basically

all the biological systems of our body to strongly impact our immediate and long term

health.

And those probiotics and Athletic Greens are optimal and vital for microbiotic health.

In addition, Athletic Greens contains a number of adaptogens, vitamins, and minerals that

make sure that all of my foundational nutritional needs are met and it tastes great.

If you'd like to try Athletic Greens, you can go to athleticgreens.com slash Huberman,

and they'll give you five free travel packs that make it really easy to mix up Athletic

Greens while you're on the road and the car on the plane, etc.

And they'll give you a year supply of Vitamin D3K2.

Again, that's athleticgreens.com slash Huberman to get the five free travel packs and the

year supply of Vitamin D3K2.

I started off studying the visual system, and I don't want this to turn into a discussion

about the visual system, but in the visual system, we know that there's a what's called

a hierarchical organization where the eye encodes and can respond to edges and light

versus dark and red, green, blue, and from that very basic set of building blocks, there's

an elaboration or a buildup of what's really called the iceberg model that was developed

by my scientific great grandparents, David Hubel and Torrance and Wiesel, who won the

Nobel Prize for that work, where you can look at somebody's face and recognize it or see

a profile moving at a particular direction and still recognize that person or see a word

written and conceptualize in your mind's eye what that word like bird actually looks like,

like parakeet, blue parakeet.

In other words, there's a hierarchical buildup and what you're describing sounds somewhat

similar that there's a hierarchical organization whereby through development, we first learn,

I guess earlier I called them primitives, but basic building blocks of, you know, when someone

steps on my foot, it hurts, can hurt a lot or a little bit depending on who stepped on

my foot, whether or not I have a shoe on, so you start learning context, but that there's

a buildup on top of the basic somatic experience of different examples that map to pain, including

emotional pain and physical pain because we know those are interdigitated somewhat, and

that over time, this builds up so that we have countless examples, but you add it, you

said something else that goes beyond the hierarchical organization that we see in the visual system,

which is that when there's a narrative or a story that we have to add, it changes something

about the representation of emotion.

I'm so struck by this comparison between seeing somebody step off a curb and break their ankle,

like even as I'm describing just like a folding ankle, like ouch, God, that really hurts.

And just look at what you're doing with your face, let's you do that in your body, right?

Yeah, I mean, I've broken my left foot five times growing up doing the same sport, and

just I can still hear and feel the thing going, and that means six months in a cast or whatever

it is, versus a story, you know, seeing somebody sitting alone in a cafe, writing in their journal,

and then you learning that they just lost their spouse of 75 years, two fundamentally different

visual images, the emotion could perhaps be the same like, oh, yes, that is rough.

And yet the need to impose story changes.

Do I understand that correctly?

That there's something not just more developmentally mature about adding in story and adding context,

but that when we have to do that, that there's something that's fundamentally different about

how the emotions are mapped in the brain.

I guess perhaps the answer I'm looking for is, what did you see in brain scanning experiments

where somebody views simply a physical break of somebody's limb versus somebody has to add

story? Is there something that comes out in the subtraction of one from the other that tells us,

oh, there's a whole set of brain networks that are not just about saying ouch, but that have to do

with the need to conjure up story and what are those brain areas and then perhaps we can digest

those a little bit.

Yes, and actually that is exactly what we found, a whole system of brain areas that did this,

which now many people have described.

And we're still trying to understand the full role of these networks, but these regions together

are called in the literature the so-called default mode network because the co-activation

of these characteristic regions of the brain, which are in the back middle of the head and

some characteristic regions in the lateral parietal.

Those were first described in neuroimaging experiments where people were asked to just

rest, rest and relax, don't think about anything, just clear your mind for a few minutes.

This is Marcus Rakel and his colleagues back in 2001.

And then contrasting that with tasks where people have to do something very

attention-focused requiring where you really have to work hard and think.

And they found that these highly metabolic characteristic regions of the brain were

coming online and activating themselves when the person was resting and deactivating and

decoupling from one another, not talking back and forth and exchanging signal very much,

when someone was doing a really effortful mental task.

And that was a real conundrum for a long time.

And what we now know is when you ask somebody to think about nothing or rest,

for a few minutes you're laying in the skin and thinking about nothing,

thinking about nothing, and then you start daydreaming about all manner of stories.

You start to imagine yourself into the future, here I am, winning the Olympics,

ta-da! Or, hey, it's my grandma's birthday next week, I wonder if she'd like to go to

lunch or if she'd rather have flowers. You're imagining other people's mind states,

you're thinking, it's like I'm out of me at work, or I wonder if I should change jobs.

You're thinking about all kinds of possible spaces that don't actually physically exist

in the real here and now. And so what we found is that our findings were, I think, some of the first,

if not the first, to actively demonstrate an increase in activation in these default mode systems,

not a decoupling of them, but an activation of them when we ask somebody to do an effortful

mental task. And what was the task? Asking people, how do you feel about this story,

which involves a lot of imposing of cultural and social and contextual knowledge to be able

to appreciate? So the story of the guy sitting in the cafe, writing in his journal, who lost his

spouse of 75 years. You have to know a lot to be able to appreciate how he must be feeling.

How does it make you feel? Let me pull up a lot of relevant knowledge, personal experiences,

and memories, and then hypothesize, generate some kind of narrative, some kind of storyline

that would accommodate his situation and allow me to infer those kinds of stories,

which are very different from, here's somebody stepping off the curb. Ow, look at that ankle,

right? It's very obvious how that makes the person feel and how you should feel about that.

You don't really need to bring a whole lot of cultural knowledge about their

personal history with their spouse to be able to understand that's breaking your ankle hearts,

right? And what we found is that it was those kinds of stories where people had to bring a

lot of contextual knowledge to fully appreciate that activated these default mode systems.

The losing of the spouse. The losing of the spouse. So what we later showed in a series of

experiments contrasting true stories that are meant to induce admiration for skill, right?

Like something physically skillful. Somebody can or cognitively skillful and memorize a Rubik's cube

and solve it with your eyes closed, right? Or do flips on your bicycle and land on a railroad

tie, right? Like these incredibly skillful things as compared to the same kind of basic

emotion in the sense of feeling like inspired, like attracted to it, like it's pleasurable,

like it's really cool, like you wish you could do that too. But now it's about a state of that

person's mind or quality of character or disposition of self. So talking about the incredibly brave

actions of Malala in Pakistan standing up to the Taliban, right? Where it's not about

how well she walks down the street holding her schoolbook. There's nothing really physically

skillful to see there. It's about the conditions under which she's doing it and what you can infer

about her state of mind and her quality of character to be engaging in these actions

under those conditions. And those complex kinds of inferences we found activate these default

mode systems uniquely. And in fact, we can in trial by trial experiment. So literally,

depending on what you say about a story, whether it inspires you that particular story out of 50,

right? In a two hour interview beforehand, if you are inspired by a particular story as compared

to another one, which may not resonate with you, right? Then when we put you in the MRI scanner,

we can predict that you will actually activate these neural systems differentially based on

your psychological reaction in the interview. So we can actually show that there are systematic

ways in which these large scale networks of the brain, so the way in which the brain is kind of

balancing its activity and its crosstalk around the different parts that are contributing different

kinds of processing, those dynamic balances are different when someone is what we're doing,

what we're calling now transcending the situation of that person, right? And starting to learn

something bigger about what it all means or what the story is or the broader reason why this inspires

me and not just is about her, right? So you can look at Malala and you can say, you know, oh,

I hope she makes it. That's really unfair and like, right? Or you can look at her and say,

and kids say this to us and experiments with teenagers, but wait a minute and they actually

wait, they cover their face, they close their eyes, they look away from the Malala video and they

look at the plain ceiling and we can actually get coders with the volume off to identify these

periods of time and say that when they come back from that pause, their speech slows, their posture

closes, right? They put their hands down, that kind of thing, they don't gesture, right? And when

they come back from that, they are talking about two things. They're talking about the broader

inferential narrative around what all this means. Wait, I didn't know not everybody in the world

doesn't get to go to, you know, gets to go to school. You know, that's not right, right? And

these ethical interpretations, that's not right. And the third thing that comes up is a feeling

of self and what it means for you because you're using your own self and consciousness as a kind

of springboard, like a trampoline, like we said before, to try to appreciate what it must be like

to be heard. So the next thing people say to us, or kids say to us especially, is it makes me realize

that I go to school all the time and I kind of take it for granted and maybe I should work harder

to try to do something about that for other people, you know? So we have this incredible

confluence in the brain and mind, this layering of real physical actions and things that happen

that you can directly observe with the visual system, right, in the world. And then you impose

upon those a desire to construct a story or meaning and you elaborate that meaning. And in doing so,

you also ramp up the internal sense of self-awareness of me being me, of conscious systems, systems

that support consciousness in the brain and brain stem, very basic things we share with

alligators, right, that become that kind of inspired state of, you know, like, wait,

it makes me want to do more for the world or it makes me inspired to know there are people

like her or she gives me hope for humanity, one kid told me, right? So we've got this incredible

dynamic layering of the feeling of the body, the real physical body, the observation and

sensation perception of the world around us in a physical real or social real sense,

and then the elaboration of that into these cultural narratives that become feeling states

and where valence kinds of disappears, right? It doesn't matter so much anymore, whether it's

painful or pleasurable, it's more about, does it mean something? I'm suffering because it's helping

someone else, right? And so it becomes something desirable even though it hurts me, right?

Otherwise, none of us would go through childbirth, right? And so it's that meaning process

that makes us really uniquely human, and that is the development of these emotions over time,

I think. Incredible. If I'm understanding correctly, there's a feeling state in our body

when we experience or observe somebody in their own feeling state or experience.

It may be the same as theirs, might be different. And frankly, as a neuroscientist,

I'm going to say, we'll never know exactly. That's the age old philosophical debate.

We won't know. If I see blue and you see blue, is it the same experience, right?

It's probably not, based on, from my knowledge of color vision and the distribution of cones,

to explain why I'm saying that, the distribution of cone photopigments in your eye and my eye

are extremely different to the point where we're not working with the same palette.

And I think that makes life interesting. That makes life interesting, exactly.

But assuming that neither of us is colorblind, red is similar enough to both of us,

that we would both look at it and say, that's red. But one in 80 males is

red-green colorblind, we'd look at it and would see what you and I call red and call it orange.

In any event, when we, let's say, listen to or watch and listen to Martin Luther King's

classic, I have a dream speech. Or when I hear certain music that I first heard when I was 14,

it was a particularly interesting for me, time in my life, in part because I was 14.

And we'll get back to that and what I mean by that.

14 is a thing.

We'll talk about adolescence, right? I'll just say, I'll go on record by saying that the,

I think that the music that we listen to in our adolescence and teen years

is one of the main ways in which we come to recognize the extremes of these feeling state

templates that you're describing. I can, one of the ways I prepare for podcasts is to walk and,

and for my solo podcast, is to walk and go through some of the narrative. My neighbors

think I'm crazy, but that's okay. I think they're crazy too.

Maybe they're both right.

That's right. Exactly. And, but I always know what music to listen to before I do a solo podcast,

depending on the state that I happen to be in, driving into the studio versus the one I need

to be in, in order to deliver that particular material. And I know because I, it's almost

like knowing what palette of colors, emotional colors I have in me at the moment, and which

ones are going to be required to deliver that material because it's different depending on

the topic matter for that episode. What I'm referring to here is, is this idea that,

you know, we, we come to understand emotions through our own experience and how observing

other people and listening to certain music can influence that. And I, I realize that some people

probably have more of a buffer between their experience of the outside world, so called

exteroception, seeing things outside us and their internal landscape. Some people, I realize,

have very little narrative distancing. In fact, I live with someone who has very little narrative

distancing. When she watches a movie, if the person gets punched, she flinches. If it's a

happy movie, she gets happy. If somebody in a movie is sad, she really feels it. And for a while,

I thought, goodness, you know, this is like, really seems a little extreme, but I've talked to

professionals about this and it's something called lack of narrative distancing.

Yeah. Transportation is another way to say it. Being transported by story.

Right. And, and I think that it has its adaptive utility. I'm not being critical. I think that's

an incredibly interesting aspect to ourselves. Some of us, I have a lot more narrative distancing,

especially with violence. And I, I think that's because I grew up around a lot more violence

than she did. And so I see somebody, you know, get beheaded in a film. And I, and I, unless it's

something where I've really been built into the story of that person, and it was a real world

thing that I knew actually happened, then I, I just kind of go, okay, well, it's a movie.

You know, there's a movie, it's not real. Even if it's a movie about something that was real,

that might be a little bit more of an emotional impact. And of course, if it's a documentary

in its real footage, it's pretty rough. But I don't, I'm not horrified in the, in the way that

she's horrified. I'm horrified, but not to the same extent. So obviously that some of us have

more of a buffer than others. And you can see this in a movie, or in a classroom full of kids

watching a speech, like the I have a dream speech, or hearing the Rosa Parks story, for instance,

or listening to and watching a David Goggins social media post, which I met David earlier,

because your son had a question for me about David Goggins. I happened to have the good fortune of

having met and know a little bit. I don't know him very well, but I know him from some in-person

interactions. And he is every bit as intense and every bit as serious about his ongoing progression

as he appears to be. There's, there's no false. So there it is 100% data fact.

He does what he claims to do and more that we don't hear about. Super impressive human being.

So when we see something like a David Goggins post, or we watch and listen to that, I have a dream

speech, and we start to feel something like, Whoa, we're feeling inspired to use the basic language.

Are we mapping to some subconscious awareness of that in ourselves? Meaning, are we mapping to

some time when we felt inspired in another circumstance? Or are we really, you know,

is this merely a kind of a return to a feeling state that we have to recognize? I don't know

if experiments have ever been done on this, but is there any way to determine whether or not

we can truly have novel emotions past age 15? Or are we really just returning, or are we really

just doing a sort of template matching of, wow, I'm feeling this again. And this makes me feel

capable, like I knew out and run today, even though I was going to basically not run today.

Or, you know, it's possible to have a fantasy view about how the world could be in terms of

equality and opportunity. And you know what? That's subconsciously is my brain saying,

yeah, I remember when I was six, and I didn't know the difference between some people having

opportunity and other people not having opportunity. Is that what's happening? Or do you think that we

are more sophisticated than that? And we are actually really responding to what we think we're

responding to. Okay. So wow, there's a lot in there. A couple of things to start. So the first

thing I was thinking before when you were talking about the visual system, which I think is relevant

now, is that as humans, the more developed we get, the more experience we have, the more we've

adapted to the contexts in which we live, you know, the real physical context, in this case,

the visual context included, but also the cultural values of that context, the things we've

noticed other people notice, right? How do you learn when you're living in the jungle that when

you see eyeballs, you should, you know, go stand next to your mommy, right? So you learn what to

notice, you learn what you need to attend to in the world. And you're, so when we are perceiving

things, either very basic things like a visual scene or hugely complex, elaborate things like

Martin Luther King's speech, we are as much imposing onto the world our own expectations

of what is there as we are perceiving what's actually there, right? So as we impose onto the

world, we bring what you might call our cultural ways of seeing and knowing our values and beliefs,

and we push them onto the experience of what we notice. So even in very basic ways,

things like cultural values change the way in which people observe and remember scenes, right?

So, you know, there's classic work by Shinobu Kitayama and other people showing that in Japan

versus in the US, when you show people a scene of, you know, like an underwater scene with like

all the beautiful things that are underwater, rocks and plants and things and little fish

swimming by and then one big fish swimming by, right? And you ask a Japanese person,

what's this picture of? They tend to talk about it's a scene of rocks and plants and little fish,

and then a big fish swims by. If you ask an American, a Western educated person, what is this

picture of? They say, oh, it's a fish swimming through a scene, right? We tend to notice first,

and he's shown that this is, you know, is very, very automatic. It's very low level. It's perceptual,

not just conceptual, and it actually changes what people actually notice in the scene and

what they remember later and all that kind of stuff, right? We learn how to sort of filter input.

We're not little, you know, robots or little video cameras walking around observing the world.

And so when we see something as complex as a social story, we impose onto that all kinds

of personal experiences. So you said, are we ever able to experience new emotions after age 15?

I think no, but we are very well able to experience new feelings, right? Which are the

complex elaborations of these physiological states and the stories we tell ourselves about the

meaning behind them, that is developing all the time. And it's developing through all kinds of

cognitive media. We do it through our science, right? By being inspired and interested in something,

by being in awe of something. We do it through art, through trying to express an emotion or a

feeling or a value state through the way in which we portray something to other people, right?

As humans, we are driven. I mean, even as cave people, we were driven to say, I was here. Here's

my handprint. I'm going to spit it onto a rock. So forevermore, anybody else comes in here is going

to see that it was me who was here. And I have a me, right? And so what we're really doing is

moving through the world, not in this kind of receptive, passive way, but we are actively

imposing ourselves onto the world. We're actively bringing our interpretive power and adapting

what we do next relative to the way in which we accommodate, right? Piaget talked about this 100

years ago, accommodate or assimilate those things into us that may disagree with our schema,

that may align and accord and reinforce them. So this matters a lot for the ways that humans

experience the world more broadly because think about, for example, a terrible topic like genocide

or the Holocaust, right? How does something like that happen, right? How is it that people who have

empathy, who love their family, who love their neighbors can suddenly turn on each other, right?

What's happened is they've shifted the way in which they narratize the context of those events.

The way in which they impose interpretation on somebody else's pain has been fundamentally

shifted from that's another human suffering to that's not a human. That's a rat, a pig, a bug,

whatever it is, right? And that dehumanization process allows us to shift our story set so that

we bring another set of values and beliefs into the space. Can I just say, I'm glad that you

brought up that dark example because my understanding from my psychology courses and university

were that as much as we would all like to think that we are incapable of being the

committers of genocide, that there are studies that were done in the 50s but then have been

repeated over many decades showing that in certain contexts, essentially everybody and anybody

would respond to an authoritarian figure and torture somebody else. And I'm sure as people

are listening to this, they're thinking, no, I would absolutely not do that. But all the data

points to the fact that if the conditions were set in a particular way, you and I and everybody

else most certainly would. A very eerie idea that goes back to I think Jung's idea that we have all

things inside of us. And we certainly have all the neural circuitry components inside of us

for rage and contempt and horrible mistreatment of others as well as all the good stuff.

But I'm just glad that you brought up this example because I think that for a lot of people,

it's inconceivable. But I've never heard it framed the way that you're describing it, which is that

if the story becomes not about the other person's suffering, but primarily about one's own story of

suffering and that can suppress or literally inhibit the neural circuits that invoke empathy,

then it makes perfectly good neurobiological sense as to why that would at least be possible.

And of course, I don't think it's a good thing. It's just like many aspects of our biology and

psychology just happens to be the way things are. It is. And I think it really, I think,

I mean, I'm the ever the optimist. I'm also ever the educator, right? I'm a teacher. I'm very also

very interested in the ways that we design educational experiences for young people.

I think the only hope we have to protect ourselves against these possibilities is to

systematically develop dispositions in ourselves, proclivities within ourselves,

to question our own motives and to deconstruct our own assumptions about situations and to engage

with other people's perspectives systematically. And when we develop those dispositions,

the hope is that we are developing within ourselves a kind of a veto system, right? A system

for checking our own motivations against other people's experiences of those motivations.

And so much of what's leading, I think, now we're going in another direction and kind of a

political direction, but so much what's leading us into these very divisive political times,

for example, not just the rise of authoritarianism, not just in the US or the threat of it, not just

in the US, but around many places in the world, all of which, by the way, are Western educated,

is that we are taught that to know something means you own something in yourself, and then you take

that with you and you impose it on the world forever more. I know how to do algebra two,

and I can do it whenever you ask me kind of thing. And that's what a good student is,

where when people learn to engage with their own knowledge states in more curious, open-minded,

flexible ways, then we dispositionally teach ourselves to check our assumptions, to rethink

what we think we know, and, and this is key, developmentally, to notice when we need to do

that and when we should just plow ahead and it's totally fine. And so what we're doing, I think,

right now to ourselves, both in the education system and in things like social media, is we're

reinforcing our own biases by diving down rabbit holes where you rehear the same thing over and

over again that reinforces your own belief systems, and then you come to believe those things,

and those put you on a train toward a particular kind of action or belief system that never

becomes deconstructed, and it's very comfortable and it's easy to do. But the responsibility,

I think, we have as individuals and as groups, as humans, given the amazing intelligence we have,

is to rise above that and actually look back on our own selves reflectively and deconstruct

our preferences, deconstruct our values and our beliefs, and systematically query them

specifically around how they impact or influence or, or, or change the situations of those around

us or don't, right, the situations and sustainability of the world that supports us or don't. And so

it all comes back to the emotions that drive our thinking. So we have these very basic

primitive physiological states which vary across individuals, the degree to which they are,

you know, incredibly powerful, easily evoked versus not, you know, there's a lot of range in

that, and all of that variation makes things interesting, right? But it's our ability to

learn to experience those and to know, develop wisdom around when we need to query our own

emotions and deconstruct the narratives that are, that we're using to validate or substantiate those

kinds of emotions in order to assess whether we actually are right, whether we should continue,

or whether we should step back and, and, and, and reframe, right? And so that kind of mental

flexibility really comes out of an emotional disposition. It is our ability, so it takes

it back to what you were asking at the very beginning, it is our ability to not just drive

from what feels like the bottom up, which of course is always starting in the top down because

you've got some interpretation of the world that makes you feel fear, that makes your body do this,

that makes you right, but also to be able to rise above, to transcend, and think about what are the

broader systemic, historical, ethical, civic implications of this narrative I'm telling myself,

which feels default like the truth, and how might I deconstruct those systematically, and how might I

invite others to give me their version of those events and engage with those systematically in

order to be able to really appreciate the implications of my beliefs. And so the bottom line is that

the emotions that we're talking about today are actually the fundamental drivers of all of our

thinking, decision-making, relationship-building, right? Our community lives and our personal

well-being all in one mix, but that doesn't kind of excuse us for acting on their bequest.

It actually imbues us with a responsibility to then develop dispositions to systematically

query those and reframe them when they are not serving us or the world well.

Exactly what you said. Yeah, so much so that, you know, I'm a big believer in following lots of

different types of social media accounts. I've taken some heat here and there because people

automatically assume that if you follow an account that you subscribe to that ideology,

but I follow many accounts through my disagree with what they say specifically so that I can

learn different perspectives. As far as I know, we're the same species, me and these other people.

Yes, as far as we are. Sometimes I wonder, but they probably wonder the same about me.

They wonder too. There's enormous range in those accounts that I follow.

I follow different accounts for different reasons, some for entertainment, some for

information, some for challenging myself, some for my desire to be baffled every now and again,

but to always return to this idea that we are all basically working with the same

building blocks of neurons and neurochemistry, some people's dopamine, which whether or not

you're into Bitcoin or traditional currency, the one true currency that's universal is dopamine.

Everyone's working for dopamine and exchanging their own dopamine with world experiences,

but this is one of the reasons why I think it's important to not be siloed in one's thinking or

exposure to different things on social media. A somewhat controversial statement, actually,

because I think a lot of people assume that if you follow somebody from a particular political

party, then that means that you vote that political party, et cetera. But that, to me,

always seemed crazy. I'm fortunate to have a good friend who is on this podcast, Rick Rubin,

who's an extremely accomplished music producer and he's produced music from essentially every

genre of music, punk rock, which is what I serve. I got my start and still love punk rock music so

much, but classical and hip hop and everything in between and Rick is somebody who forages so

broadly. And I've really learned to try and forage broadly in terms of ideas and ideologies.

I think a lot of people are just scared to be exposed to something that they hate so much

because they don't like that feeling in their body of disagreement.

Yeah, dissonance is very... That kind of cognitive dissonance, we call it, is very

difficult. It takes work to resolve it. Yeah. I like to think there's a way to step back from

that and observe it, not from a disconnected stance, but from a place of curiosity about what's

driving those mechanisms in people. And maybe where we need to adjust our thinking, maybe not

to adopt their mode of thinking 100%, but maybe 10% or 2%. I think one of the reasons

things are so divisive right now is because of social media and this siloing or very

divergent trajectories of people only following and listening to and obeying certain kinds of

information and other people, the other. And I think the pandemic is the place where all that

really clashed very heavily and continues to clash in other areas too. Certainly not something

that's going to be solved inside of this conversation. And yet, I do have a question that

grows from this aspect of our discussion, which is, what do you think can be done at a concrete level

in terms of education of younger people as well as education of people who are out of high school

and beyond to try and adopt these more encompassing modes of learning and experiencing the world?

I mean, it's one thing to say, expose yourself to lots of different ideas.

It's another to understand how to do that in a way that is adaptive. And any ideas you have,

I think I know I and the audience would really appreciate and feel free to

make this an editorial or map back to data. I mean, obviously, this is your wheelhouse,

this is your expertise. So I'm curious, what should we do? Should I send my family members who

have very divergent political beliefs from me, information to the contrary they're thinking,

what do I do? And what do I do for me? What should we all be doing with our 10-year-olds

and ourselves? Well, I won't comment on should you send your family members, there's other people

that do that and they do that work and they know how to. We're always frustrating each other over

text messages. It's okay. It's okay. It can't get any worse. We all love each other anyway.

But one thing I really do think a lot about in this is the way in which we educate our young

people and what do we do with our 10-year-olds? The first thing I'll say about your 10-year-olds,

I don't know if you actually have a 10-year-old, is query them about their beliefs. When they

follow something, when they think something's impressive or bad or ask them why, teach them

to unpack their own beliefs. That doesn't mean that you don't still hold them necessarily. It

doesn't mean that you adopt the opposite belief. If I talk to someone who has a very different

value system than I do and I disagree with them, that's legitimate. But in deciding that I disagree,

I have sort of revisited my own belief and queried it. I've externalized it a little bit,

made that thinking visible is the way we talk about it in education. That's David

Perkins at Harvard talks about it that way. Making your thinking visible and then examining that

thinking. I think one really important step that a society will have to take or we won't make it,

and I know that sounds a little dramatic, but I actually think it's true, sadly,

and I'm starting to think it's more and more true, is that we need to really get brave

about how we think about the process of educating our young people and what it

actually means to expose young people to developmentally appropriate, age-appropriate

opportunities to grow themselves as thinkers, as individuals, and as civic agents and community

members. I think that our western-designed education system has in it some very basic

beliefs about what counts as knowing and what is worth thinking about and knowing about,

and how do I know that, how do I test you on that, that I think they are deeply problematic

and lead us. I know this is a strong statement, but they lead us to a place where we are actively

punished, not just not encouraged, but I would say actively discouraged from

really playing with ideas, engaging systematically with our own beliefs, deconstructing those

beliefs, and engaging with complex perspectives on topics and ideas. That is just not what school

is about, and it needs to be. We need to shift. Right now, the way in which we think about

school is basically judged by quote-unquote learning outcomes. What have you learned,

and how do we know that? We make you demonstrate it by yourself under time pressure in a particular

setting, or you're going to come back and I'm going to give you a question, and you're going

to give you the answer I had in mind. If you do that in time, then I'll say you learned it,

and now we're done. Check. As compared to a system, and there are educational systems like this,

there are people, for example, the performance assessment consortium in New York City is a

consortium of public schools, some of which do this extraordinarily well. They have a

dispensation from the New York State government not to give the Regents Exam as their graduation

requirements and their benchmarks of learning, but instead to have alternative ways of assessing

kids, where kids work for months to years, depending on the project, on these in-depth,

intellectual, multidisciplinary projects where they explore a topic, and they engage with their own

process of learning about that topic, and they bring in teachers and community experts and other

people, and they present their work, and then they query the work, and they talk about their own

learning process, and what could happen next, and what decisions they made, and all these kinds

of graduate thesis. Exactly. You have to invent not just the work, but the question. You need to

look at the world and notice what it is we're not understanding that we would benefit from

understanding, and find a way to isolate and systematically query that. Why don't we build

education systems from preschool all the way up that engage people systematically in that

kind of intellectual curiosity? We don't do that. We know that little kids' education, preschool

education, if you don't have the water table and the sand table and the cool stuff and the choices

and the ways to engage with each other and all the stuff being really age-appropriate for three-year-olds

to touch and smush and try to taste and whatever else, they're going to be a mess on the floor.

They're just not going to come. They're going to refuse to come to school, and they're going to be

laying in the doorway throwing temper tantrums. We know how to do little kid education well.

It doesn't mean we always do it, but we know that they need to be intrigued. They need to be invited

to think, and they bring their natural curiosity, and then you expand the range of ways they can

leverage that curiosity to discover new things they hadn't known to think about before.

Right? Then we get to the standard, quote unquote, educational system, and we somehow think that

that natural human proclivity to engage curiously and meaningfully with deep thinking about ideas

and the world is like inefficient and inappropriate and frightening, and we teach kids, no, no, no,

turn that off. It's dangerous. If you do it, it's considered insubordinate, right?

And what we want you instead to do is just let me give you what I've already figured out for you.

I'm going to give it to you, and you are going to give it back to me.

And so it seems to me that in the way that things actually happen in school, what is created is

a kind of desire for the kid to be a computer, not a human. And they do have a dopamine system,

however. And so what becomes the buzz, the emotional buzz is performance. If it becomes a buzz at all.

So for the kids that don't get that buzz from performance, or they don't intrinsically love

the math or the English or the books that they're being presented with or whatever the subject

happens to be, or maybe they only like one or two things, then they emotionally dissociate

from the rest of the material. I'm actually describing a bit of myself in high school.

I was not, I barely finished high school. I dropped out of sixth grade for a few months.

Yeah, didn't work for me. Yeah, you know, I eventually got back to it. And as I imagine you

did too, we ended up as academics. But I think what you're describing is so key. And I never

thought about it from the perspective of, oh, yeah, as young kids, like we're given all the

things that are going to drive our sensory world in the appropriate ways, touch and sound and

and a mind, right? We're trying to build meaning in our mind.

And that we get to, as students, young, very young learners, impose some of our own

intrinsic motivation to do certain things and not others. And that that isn't supported as we're

adults. I'd like to take a quick break and thank our sponsor, Inside Tracker. Inside Tracker is

a personalized nutrition platform that analyzes data from your blood and DNA to help you better

understand your body and help you reach your health goals. I have long been a believer in

getting regular blood work done for the simple reason that many of the factors that impact

your immediate and long term health can only be assessed with a quality blood test. The problem

with a lot of blood and DNA tests out there, however, is that they'll give you information about

certain lipid markers or hormone markers, but no information about what to do with all of that data.

Inside Tracker makes it very easy to look at your levels of hormones, metabolic factors,

lipids, et cetera, and then to assess what sorts of behavioral nutritional supplementation or perhaps

other interventions you might want to use in order to bring those numbers into the ranges that are

optimal for your health. Inside Tracker's ultimate plan now includes three new hormone markers that

are critical to measure during a woman's reproductive and menopausal years. These are

estradiol, progesterone and thyroid stimulating hormone. If you'd like to try Inside Tracker,

you can go to insidetracker.com slash huberman to get 20% off any of Inside Tracker's plans.

Again, that's insidetracker.com slash huberman to get 20% off.

What you're describing is so vital. What age do you think this

cliffs off? Okay, so in preschool, kids are allowed to do this. In kindergarten,

they're allowed to do it. First grade, they're allowed to do it in most schools, but at what

point is the expectation imposed on kids to become little rote learning computer machines

and to get their dopamine from performance rather than from intrinsic pleasure in what

they're learning? And also, how do we address this issue that there are certain basic skills that

not everyone is going to perform well at? And so for the kid that says, I don't like math.

Well, you still have to learn it. You need to appreciate it.

How do you conjure up a joy or an appreciation in that kid? It seems like a hard thing. I mean,

I eventually set myself along a academic trajectory that worked out. But that was initially just

out of pure fear because my life was really bad. I made circumstances and myself made it bad.

And I was rescuing myself from basically becoming more of a loser. So I was like,

okay, school is the thing and I did school. And that was the turn hard right into academics for

me. But what do you do for the person who really doesn't like math because they're struggling with

it or doesn't like biology or psychology? How do we evoke at least an appreciation for that?

It sounds like the emotion system is the key system to leverage in order to learn.

And so could you talk about the relationship between emotion and learning? Because I realize

this is really the center of what you do. So I mean, you could say it this way, right? So whatever

you're having emotion about is what you're thinking about, right? And whatever you're thinking about,

you could hope to learn about, remember something from, right? Understand differently.

So the key question for educators is what everybody's always having some kind of emotions

all the time, unless you're dead, right? Or unconscious. What are people's emotions about

in this space? If the emotions, because whatever those emotions are about, that is what you're

learning about. So if the emotions are about the outcomes, did I get it right? Am I going to flunk?

Did I get an A plus? I'm so smart. I'm so stupid. Well, any one of those, right? If those are the

main drivers, then that is what you're learning about. If the emotions are about the actual ideas

in play, the math, the physics, the why does the ball roll down the ramp? Wait a minute,

that's the same as why the moon goes around. You know what I mean? Like, there are, right?

When the emotions are about ideas, then what you're engaging with is learning about ideas.

And so what I would argue is that in setting up the kind of accountability system we have,

we have taught people that their emotion should be about these high stakes accountability

measures, which means that's what we're learning how to think about. Perform. Perform. Not how to

think about the ideas, not the intrinsic power of using math to understand the world in a different

way. So how do you engage kids, right? You engage kids by setting out rich problem spaces

that in problems that invite them to try to engage with something that peaks your curiosity that's

meaningful to them or have them bring in where the kid who really hates it, like what is it that

you do find interesting kid, right? Start there. Start there and start using your academic skills

in a way that will give you power to do what it is you're interested in doing. That's the way in.

Use your writing, use your math, use your persuasive argument skills, use your filmmaking

skills, whatever it is, to tell the story of something that you find deeply, meaningfully

powerful to understand. And all of a sudden, you need the math. Kids actually say things like,

like, there's this lovely, there's this lovely long quote from a Sudanese immigrant kid in one

of these New York schools with the performance assessments. In an article I wrote with a colleague

named Doug Connect, the article is called Building Meaning Builds Teen's Brains. You can find it in

educational leadership. There's a big long quote from this kid at the end, and he's basically

explaining what math class meant to him, which he had never passed a math class before. And he says,

he got this problem called walking to the door, which is basically Zeno's paradox, right? You

get halfway to the door, halfway to the door, halfway to the door. Do you ever get to the door?

Why or why not, right? And they spent months learning the math that would help them get

out that problem. And he talks about how I had a problem, he says, and I had to learn fractions.

I had to in order to be able to solve the problem I had. And as I engaged with fractions and that

problem, I got fascinated, he says, by finite and infinite. And these ideas were driving my need

to learn to do fractions, right? So we've got the cart before the horse. I'm not saying you

don't have to learn math or you don't have to learn to read or write or do all these other

kinds of skills. But we make those, which is in the horse's cart, you know, what's in the cart,

we call that the metric of the education system and the aim of it, when in fact it's the quality

of the horse. Can that horse pull the thing, right? That's the development of the person.

And what they put in their cart then serves that development. It's the toolkit of ways of knowing

and understanding that come with you as you move into the world. But this takes real,

real developmental skill on the part of educators, right, who are not supported or

resourced or trained to think about development in these ways. I mean, so you asked, when does

this fall off? It really depends on what school system you are and in what demographic you are

when it falls off. But for almost everybody, except for the privileged few who are in very

progressive alternative schools, it falls off by adolescence, which is when school gets serious

and is also ironically, when developmentally, kids are developing the neural capacity and the

psychosocial capacity and the drive to infer complex narrative meaning from the things they

are doing. You know, these aren't just my shoes. These are a statement about, you know, what I

believe about sustainability and about sports and about adults and counterculture, right?

And as we grow into a space where we're driven to try to, you know, challenge and think about

big meanings and engage with perspectives and emotions and social issues and broad,

important existential questions, be they in physics or be they in art or be they in the

social civic domain, right? What do we do? We double down on controlling the input and the

output transactional mechanisms that count as quote-unquote academic rigor and achievement,

right? We start to ask kids, you know, what's the name of the servant who shows up in the scene

and great expectations, right? Is it Molly or is it Maria, right? And it's, you know, like who the

heck knows? And that is not the point of leading great expectations, right? We take away because

we're afraid. As educators, as society, we've got this narrative around young peoples in particular,

but everyone's propensity to build and construct meaning in these spaces and self in these spaces,

that agency frightens us because we're worried they're going to take risks, they're going to do

something stupid, they're going to, they're going to fall off the track, they're going to not make

it in the traditional system. And in trying to protect them and shield them from their own

curiosities, their own dispositions for meaning making, we, I would argue, actually stunt their

ability to grow themselves to the point where we have mental health crises, literally crises in

mental health right now in adolescence, across demographic groups. Especially bad in young

girls, as I understand. Yes, that's right. But bad in everybody and it's worse in girls. Yes,

we don't fully understand why that is. I've got some suggestions. You know, what we're really doing

is actually producing people who are gutted of their own inner drive to become someone who

thinks powerfully in the space of the world. We are frightened to let our young people have that

power, which is the role of adults is to wrap around young people and help them learn to be

reflective, to be systematic, to be rigorous with themselves as they develop the capacities

and dispositions to deconstruct their own beliefs, to deconstruct their own aims and goals and the

ways they understand the world and to rebuild them iteratively over and over in this sort of

intellectually humble, curious way where we're constantly querying ourselves, constantly querying

other people, where we're willing to sit with uncertainty in complex problem spaces and think

through the possibilities rather than settle quickly onto one solution. What does school

expect you to do? Settle immediately onto one solution, which by the way is the solution I

already had in mind when I gave you the question, right? As compared to sitting with young people and

allowing them in safe and appropriate ways the space in which to actually grapple with complex

powerful questions. When kids develop the proclivities to do that, they learn how to manage

those very human capacities that we've been talking about the whole time that can lead to,

you know, terrible, evil, as well as amazing virtuousness. They learn to appreciate and manage

those capacities within their own selves. I think so much of what we see in terms of these,

you know, quote unquote, failure to launch examples are because I know some of these,

the children of friends, really, really smart kids that didn't map well to the system

and therefore are not doing well, really struggling and clearly have the intellectual power. It just

wasn't served up to them and school wasn't served up to them in a way that

was. Yeah, that says as much about the system as it does about the kid, right? Yeah, I teach

a course at Stanford to the medical students that every first year medical student takes about

neuroscience. It's team taught. It's a phenomenal course because of the range of expertise in the

teaching that comes through. And one thing I've noticed is that they're all phenomenal teachers,

but the best instructors do two things simultaneously when they teach. First of all,

they come to the table with incredible expertise, obviously. You have to deeply understand what

you're trying to get at if you want people to engage with ideas. Yeah, they are true luminaries

in their respective fields, addiction, pain, memory, every system of the body and brain that

relates to the nervous system is taught in this course. But that I've noticed every once in a

while that there's a subset of them that as they teach from that position of expertise,

not only are they clear, not only are they engaging, not only are their slides sparse enough to

understand but rich enough to include all the relevant detail, but they also flip back and forth

from the position of expert to the position of novice learning it for the first time.

That's that intellectual curiosity that they're keeping a lot. They have this disposition we're

talking about cultivating. Sorry to cut you off. No, please do. As academics, we're familiar with

that, right? Interrupting in the landscape of academics, interrupting me is a sign of interest.

I think Carol, I think Carol Dweck was the one who told me that. Okay, she's right.

Thank you, Carol. She's right. The great Carol Dweck. And so, but I've seen this especially,

so, you know, there are some topics that, you know, I like to think that I might do this reflexively

for because, like, for instance, I started off in neural development and I adore the topic.

So I can't teach neural development without being completely blown away in the positive sense

of how a brain develops. Yeah. I've still never taught this or done a podcast on it

because it tends to require visuals and we don't use those because the podcast, most people listen

to the podcast, but maybe I'll do something just for YouTube at some point. But I think it's the

same experience occurs when I see somebody like Dr. Sean Mackie who runs our pain clinic at Stanford

teach about pain and the systems of the body that relate to pain and emotion and

how to cure certain forms of pain, et cetera, treat pain. It's like he's clearly the world expert,

but the way he describes the system, you can tell he's learning it again for the first time

in parallel to all of that. And I feel like that ignites the emotional systems of the learner's

brain in such a powerful way that is distinct from just hearing an expert talk about something.

He's not relaying. He's not a squirrel with nuts and giving all the nuts to the kids.

He's inventing the knowledge in front of them, right? That's a great way to put it. As usual,

others are more succinct in collecting my ideas and expressing them than I am.

So I think that's a powerful thing. I went to a high school that has a kind of a split

reputation. It's known as being one of the best public high schools in the country. It's also

the high school that at least for a while had one of the highest suicide rates in the country.

It's written up in various newspapers and so on. And so much so that nowadays they forbid

the kids there from meeting more than an hour before school to practice for the standardized

tests. By the way, when I was at school, the only thing that school represented for me in high school

was something that came between breakfast and skateboarding. And frankly, I wasn't in school

a lot. And I don't recommend that kids go to school, stay in school. I missed a lot of school.

Traveled all over the place and all kinds of weird things. I had a lot of making up to do in

college as a consequence. So stay in school, get the basics. But this is actually where I'd like

to go. You have a very interesting trajectory. Here you are. You're a university professor. You

study emotion and learning and many other things across cultures and adolescents and so many other

important topics. But you are not a story of growing up in an academic family. You grew up on

a farm. Sort of gentlemen's farm. My dad was a surgeon, but we had animals in a farm and my

parents tried to have us growing the things we ate. You've had a number of different experiences

that we were talking about before we started recording. But one of the things that you mentioned

was getting involved in education where you were exposed to students who had very different

backgrounds than you. Maybe you could just talk a little bit about the nodes of your experience.

So you grew up on this farm and then maybe just hit some of the other nodes and then let's take

a foray into when you first got exposed to educating others. Because I think that's an

important backdrop for what we've been talking about here and serves as a jumping off point for

I'd like to go next. I'll just jump in. It's always hard to talk about yourself. I don't know

what's interesting and what's not. It's just me. I think what's interesting is knowing where you've

been and the things that mapped back to your emotional networks in a way that for you feel

like that mattered in terms of what you're doing now. As a little kid, I remember even as a little

kid not liking school. I was a very good kid. I was a very well behaved kid. I went to a decent

public school. But just the whole idea of it, I just always felt like I had two left feet.

It never felt like it was really me there. I was always trying to escape a little bit,

you know what I mean? And thinking about when I first started educating others and like my

first memory of educating others like specifically that comes to mind is I was six and I went on a

little vacation in the summer to stay with my cousins in Petoskey, Michigan, which is a place

on Lake Michigan where there are these stones where there's my understanding from when I was six is

that there are these like 200 million year old fossilized worms in these stones. And you can

see them when you look at them. There's like little worms and you can see them. Yeah. So I

just was fascinated by these stones that these are actual fossilized 200 million year old worms.

And I don't know if that number is correct. That's what I remember from age six of some

paleontologists out there can correct me. But I collected these stones and I went to the little

local exhibit they had at the library or whatever. And I learned about these stones and I brought

some back and somehow somebody thought to ask me to teach my second grade class when I started

school about these stones. And I just remember, I don't know how I got asked to do this, but I

remember standing in front of my class and talking about these stones and just looking around the

room and suddenly noticing, you know, that feeling when you're lecturing and you think,

oh my God, they're fascinated by what I'm saying. Like every kid is looking at me and like,

holy crap, you know, like, and I was so I'm like, all right, I'll keep going. I'll tell you some

more about these stones and I passed them around and whatever. And it must have been okay because I

was then asked to give that talk all the way up to the fifth graders who were way older than me.

You're a professor. You're a professor. I was already fascinated by the natural world

and able to like make meaning out of something in a way that inspired other people if I can be so

blunt as to say that. And yet, I was constantly in trouble at school for not having my homework.

Like I was just, you know, the feeling of release on the Friday afternoon and the feeling of dread

on Sunday evening is hard to like describe, you know, and I went to a reasonably well resourced

school, you know. Anyway, fast forward up to when I was older. I mean, I was just always

fascinated by, and I think someone this comes from my mom too, trying to, you know, speak

different languages, engage with people who are different than myself, just have conversations.

So from the time I was old enough to barely qualify to do these programs, my parents had the

the resources, luckily, to be able to let me to do these things. But I, you know, I went off to

France and stayed on a farm there for a summer and went to, you know, Ireland. I went to Russia

by the time I was 18, I was working with these little kids off the street and camping with them

in southern Siberia and all these kinds of things when I was as cold as they say in Siberia. It was

gloomy and rainy and muddy and cold. Yes. Yeah. Siberia always sounds so bleak. My parents

threatened many times to send me there. Oh, yeah. No, that's the real threat. I mean, it's beautiful

in many ways, but yeah, this that was sad. It was a sad, sad story. Anyway, you know, I think

what I was trying to do was actually learn by doing, by being, by engaging with other

people who knew things I didn't, learning how to, you know, build things. I was always really

interested in warm working and boat building. I went to Kenya and spent eight months there

as an undergraduate, right? Documenting this traditional Dow construction in northern

coast of Kenya, Dow, which are sailboats, sailboat construction when they have no electricity

and everything. Cabin tree. Yeah, cabinetry. You know what I mean? You can actually build

furniture. So when people say they built furniture, but they basically assembled a

Kia furniture, we're not talking about that. Yeah, no, I have built cabinets and built in

bookshelves and furniture. Some of my friends have pieces I've made for them. I never made

anything for myself, so I don't have anything. But yeah, I mean, I think I was really torn between

trying to build things and learn by engaging with other people and in these different cultural

spaces. You know, being a woman in a cabinet shop in Connecticut is really not a cultural space

that I had grown up in and then gone, you know what I mean? And yet, right, moving myself and

changing myself to adapt to these different situations somehow felt like learning to me,

I think. And I ended up in a strange situation where I cut my hand opening a window at a job site

and I needed to, I was on workers comp and I had to take some time to let it heal

and I couldn't run machinery. So I had to figure out what to do with myself. I was 23 years old

and I was not going to go back to my parents for more money, right? So I thought I have to support

myself. So I thought, okay, I went to college at a high level. I feel like school and I majored in

French because I could. That's basically what I was like. I don't know. I better finish. I'm not

a flunk. I can do French. I speak French fluently. I'll do a French literature major and

download it quickly. Then I'm like, what am I going to do with myself? I never thought I could

be a scientist, but I love science. So I just went around taking like a year of every science.

I took a year of astronomy and a year of biology and a year of physics and a year of, you know,

human anthropology, paleoanthropology, like all these things psychology and realized, holy crap,

like this is super interesting. You can study how babies think and the natural world

and then also be bringing sort of a scientific lens to bear that helps you understand things

in a new way. So here I was as a 23-year-old with a cut hand and I thought, what am I going to do

with myself? I convinced the Massachusetts Board of Education that I had the background knowledge

to be able to teach some sections of AP biology and physics that they had in their high school.

So when I finally got an interview with this public school district in South Boston

where they were desperate for a teacher, like I'm noticing in the Boston Globe,

we're two weeks into the school year and you still don't have a teacher. You know what I mean?

Why don't you take me and manage to convince the Massachusetts Board of Education to give me

provisional teacher certification based on the coursework I'd done and how well I did in that

coursework because I was really super motivated. I did extremely well and all of it. And when I got

there, they basically said when I showed up for the interview, you know, the high school teacher

wants to take those AP classes. Can you just teach full-time seventh grade? So I was like, okay.

So I had my full contingent of 130 kids, right? Seventh graders coming through my classroom

and the middle school had just been shut down because there wasn't sufficient funding in the

town for it. So they had taken the middle school kids and pushed them into the high school space.

What that basically meant is I suddenly found myself in a fully equipped high school classroom

with microscopes and all kinds of scientific equipment that would be used to teach later

courses with my seventh graders. And it also happened that the Massachusetts Board of Education

had changed the requirements for the way they organized science instruction and curriculum

from seventh grade life science, eighth grade physical science, whatever it was,

different sciences each year. They wanted an integrated interdisciplinary science

all the way across. And of course, that was very difficult for the traditional science

teachers to do because they'd been teaching only biology or only earth science or only

physical science for their whole career and they didn't know how to teach the other subjects.

And here comes me with like one intensive year of study in each of these domains.

I was perfectly situated to like try to pull it together. So some of the high school teachers

helped me. Thank you to them. And I built out a new curriculum for seventh grade for that district

around this interdisciplinary approach to science together with other teachers.

It was very hands on.

Very and it was very much like a web of concepts, you know, we'd study nuclear fission and atoms

and reactions and then the sun and astronomy and the solar system and then and then how the energy

is being, you know, shined over onto the onto the planets and then the earth and then these

organisms called plants are actually using those photons to do something chemical. Let's talk about

photosynthesis and we're right. And then we can talk about chemical reactions and breaking down

sugars and molecules. And right. So we built this whole web like curriculum that I was trying to

help the kids appreciate the sort of dynamic complexity of the natural world. And some of my

professors from Cornell also sent me materials and all kinds of cool stuff from the Cornell Museum

that that they didn't really need. And then I gave it back when I was down right with all these

instructions, what all this stuff is on hominid evolution and actually in hand axes and all

kinds of stuff. So I built out a curriculum around all this stuff. And I realized the first

time that I was in this amazingly fascinating space, because it just so happened that the school I

was working in was one of the most diverse culturally in the nation at that time. I think we had

something like 81 languages spoken out of 1,100 kids. Wow. That's a lot of first languages.

And kids were arriving from all over the world. This was right after the Rwandan genocide. So

kids were coming in from East Africa. There were refugees from Kosovo and Eastern Europe. There

were kids coming in from Jamaica. There were kids coming in from Haiti. There were kids from Malaysia

and Myanmar. There were kids landing in that class like deer and headlights from very,

very broad ranges of cultural backgrounds. And they're landing in my science class.

And what I quickly realized is they were using these scientific ways of exploring the world and

thinking about questions and trying to make sense of what they had witnessed

to try to understand their own selves, their own origin story, their own place in the world,

why different people in this class look and eat differently than me, dress differently than me.

Like, how is it that you look like that? And I look like this. And there was all this crazy,

you know, adolescent turmoil layered into this space where kids were grabbing onto the scientific

ways of knowing as a handle to try to make sense of who they are. And those kids started asking

questions of me. I'll never forget this. One girl, a black girl raised her hand and all

the other kids are looking at her like, yeah, yeah, yeah, ask it, ask it, right? And like,

you know, she was being brave. Like, she talked about it before school. Like, I can't say that.

No, I can't say it. And she said, Miss Immordino, why is it that when we're studying

hominid evolution and you show us these, this Nova episode with early hominids in Africa,

why do they always show those creatures looking like they have dark skin? Why do they always

look like black people? And I was like, well, because they're on the equator and you need that

level of melanin in your skin to be able to adapt and live without getting skin cancer in that

space, right? And it opened up this amazing class discussion that actually went on for months,

like it evolved into a whole curriculum that was biology, it was culture, it was sociality,

where we started to really unpack the ways that we as humans are natural beings in the world and

the ways in which our cultural experiences are extensions of our natural ways of adapting.

And that had me hooked. I realized then that I could bring science, right? The science of

adolescent development and of learning and of emotion and of culture to this very pressing real

world problem of how do we help our kids actually figure out who they are, invent themselves in

this incredible, crazy multicultural space and become scholars and intellectuals who engage

systematic with the ideas along the way. And so I took those ideas and I started going to

night school at Harvard Extension School to study cognitive neuroscience and to study

language and cognition and, you know, all these kinds of topics and quickly realized, like,

I really needed this developmental perspective infused, right? I wanted to understand not just

how these things work, but how they got that way. And so I took that back to grad school

at Harvard and began to study, you know, social and cultural and emotional and cognitive development

in kids. And quickly there also kind of hit a wall where I went back to the school district in

which I worked and I went back to the teachers who were my colleagues and I worked with them and I

observed their classes and I interviewed their students and we did all kinds of work around

how kids were building scientific concepts in ways that reflected their cultural concepts and

ways of approaching the world. And I quickly realized, you know, it seems to me that kids are

doing all this meaning making and we as adults are doing all this, all this supportive, you know,

meaning making. We're also engaging and growing and learning in ways that reflect not just, you

know, knowledge bits like little computers, but also that reflect the biological substrate on

which the learning and the thinking are happening. And I wanted very much to understand how we could

use and leverage developmental biology as a kind of constraint from which to appreciate the kinds

of theoretical frames we were inventing in the real world sort of anthropological educational

space, the developmental psychological space. How could these two systems, you know, act as

a Venn diagram and how could the inner section between them, the places where the theorizing

about the natural behaviors and the way kids were making meaning and learning

and describing their knowledge and engaging with each other on the one hand and the ways in which

the brain and the biology are engaging in or supporting those processes. On the other hand,

the places where those two circles would overlap, it seemed to me that was where

we could most directly target to start to deeply understand the nature of our developmental

psychological growth and selves. And so I set out to try to study about the ways in which culture

and sociality shape the brain and physiology and survival mechanisms and development. And

at that time, which wasn't even that long ago, you know, it's like two decades ago, quickly

realized very, very little was known, you know, about the way in which emotions beyond things like

fear, you know, flash a snake in your face and your amygdala lights up, right? Like,

I was thinking of something a little more nuanced, you know what I mean? Like, what I'm seeing happening

in science class among a kid from Kosovo and a kid from Rwanda is they're trying to figure out

why they understand how they look different, right? Those deeply emotional conversations they're

having, but they're not so cut and dry as the things we had been studying. And so that's what

really drove me to try to start to understand in an integrated way the way in which our

biological development and our psychological development are actually sort of two sides

of who we are and of how we're organizing ourselves to build capacity, mental capacity,

as well as sort of physical health and capacity over the course of our lives as we're engaging

with living. Incredible story and foray into what sounds to me like really your ability to

identify how the universals among us, like the universal biological features, the universal

psychological features, can really strongly inform specifically what's happening now in a

classroom interaction in the mind of you or somebody else or of any of us, but to approach

it from the other direction. In other words, to take what's happening now and say, why is what's

happening now happening? Yeah, as opposed to just saying... What is actually happening underneath

the surface of the behavior? Right, as opposed to saying, okay, this is the psychology of character

structure. This is the biology of the hypothalamus, but rather say, is anyone else really shocked about

the school shooting in Nashville and go through the feeling of shock and go from there to the

biology as a route of learning. And of course, I don't want to take away anything from the

real-world seriousness of that, but it sounds to me like you saw that there's a different

portal through which to teach and understand, experiencing that we are all, but especially

young people, are really tied to our emotional states as the main filters, which we like that,

dislike that, and therefore make decisions and move through life. I mean, I think it's so key

that early on, I mean, if we like a teacher, oftentimes we like the subject. If we happen

to fall in love with, you know, Figure 4B in a paper, great, but that's not how I went through

graduate school. I just was blown away by the fact that sperm meets egg, you get a bunch of cell

duplications, and then you get a brain, and then you get a brain. You're like, crud, how does that

happen? That's crazy. Amazing. And I was blessed with a graduate advisor who literally told me,

this is how it works in my lab, is what she said. She said, we have everything you need here.

I'll help you if you need help, but basically you're going to mess around with stuff. You're

not going to burn down the lab. You're not going to kill yourself with any of the poisonous stuff,

but then you're going to like mess some stuff up and do some stuff, and you're going to figure

some stuff out. This is literally the description, and I liked her lab because I had green countertops,

and she had pictures of interesting animals on the wall, and then she said, and I'm going to have

two kids while you're in graduate school. So I'm not going to be around very much. You're going to

have to figure it out on your own. And I said, well, can I play the music I want? And she said,

sure. And I said, can I put tinfoil on the windows because I don't want to be bothered.

And she said, sure. And I was like, okay, this is the place for me. In other words,

she gave me a room to explore. And of course, she gave me a lot of guidance along the way.

She was an amazing, amazing graduate advisor. I'm extremely blessed. But it sounds to me like

that identifying what's really going on now is key. And that the other thing that's key is an

openness to ideas. I mean, earlier, you talked about kind of the, let's just admit where we're

at right now. We're in a culture war right now. We're in a weird space right now. It's very divisive.

And one of the major problems is that we can't really talk about things. I mean, I think fear

of getting canceled, fear of exploring ideas is real. It's very real, not just for academics,

it's just real people are so it's important to be sensitive to the experiences of others. Absolutely.

But if we can't actually explore ideas and feel like we can walk out of the room

safely, then we can't really explore ideas. And so I think right now, it's not just social media,

I think it's the fear of offending anybody. And probably the fear of voicing how upset

certain people are about their experiences or the experiences of others, whatever it is,

I don't see a landscape right now where there is true open exploration of ideas anywhere,

anywhere, at least in this country. So what do we do if at least two of the requirements are

an emotional gripping of something around the learning plus an openness to thinking about

things that maybe we don't feel right to us as a way to learn how to think something. I think we

both agree, if I may, that is really critical and that the world will be a far better place if people

could do that. And how do we navigate this landscape? I mean, is what has to come first,

a demonstration of the value of openness and ideas. And here, I'll just state my stance,

I feel like any idea should be open to at least discussion, any idea. But then it needs to be

systematically dissected with some rigor, so that people can't just assume any idea is true,

just because it's true for them. And this I actually learned from my graduate advisor,

tolerance has to go both ways. Like when it comes to thinking about ideas and criticizing,

it can't just be I'm right, they're wrong, or I don't tolerate that, it has to be

tolerance for all ideas. And then you arrive at hopefully, eventually core truths or at least

core trajectories. What do you think could support this? How early should this start? I mean,

kids in elementary school be discussing the current landscape of politics and what they see

from a place of like, we talk about safe spaces, but is a safe space one in which

no one gets offended? Or is a safe space one in which any idea can be discussed? I think

that's never really been defined for me. Yeah. Ooh, that's a really fraught issue.

I mean, first, let me go back to something you said, which I would have said it differently.

So you said our emotions are a filter, right? And they do act like a filter,

but I actually don't think emotions are really filter like so much as they are the drives

that are undergirding the impetus to think, right? They're pushing us to think about particular

things. And I think, I mean, as a scientist, my disposition is always that to understand

something is good. And the more complexly, the more thoroughly you can interrogate and

understand something, the better. So there's nothing I'm afraid of knowing, right? And what

you're really talking about there is the fear of knowing. Why are people so afraid to engage with

each other, basically, because it's deeply threatening to reveal things about your own

experience that are not going to land in a space where we can kind of collectively engage with

them as legitimate experience. That's sort of the opposite of canceling people, right? It's

the opposite of dismissing people. It's actually developing spaces of trust where we can engage

with ideas and take them from ourselves, right? So they're no longer personal value judgments.

They become cultural memes or models or schemas that we can dissect together,

that we can engage with together and construct understanding around, right? And I don't really

understand my own position unless I also understand your opposition to my position,

even if I still disagree with you. I think there are really important conversations

going on right now. I'll take it back to the education system because that's what I know

what most about. There are really important conversations going on right now around reframing

the experience and outcomes and aims of schooling around civic discourse and reasoning. So there

was just a major report that was produced by the National Academy of Education and

other academies collaborating with it, for example, around this topic. And helping us to move as a

society toward a space where we learn to kind of lay ideas out and develop skills for reasoning

around those ideas, including bringing ethical, experiential, emotional, cultural values to bear,

but then being willing to deconstruct and engage with those ideas, whether they're the ones that

are commensurate and fluid with our experience or that appear to be conflicting or dis-fluid

with our experience. We need to develop spaces for young people, especially, but for everyone,

to engage with the deconstruction of our own assumptions, like I said before, and to engage

with the deconstruction of others' assumptions and to try to reconcile the building blocks. And

that's where we can build some common ground, but we can also disagree. But we don't really

understand our own position unless we appreciate someone else's disagreement with our position,

unless we can actually articulate and appreciate how it is that person's opinion is opposed to mine,

I don't really understand mine. It's such a key point. One of the reasons why I do read all the

comments on podcasts on YouTube, it takes me some time, but I do it on social media is that

oftentimes I'll get a comment or criticism that makes it very clear that I wasn't clear about

something. Other times I'll get a comment or criticism that makes it clear that I and the

other person fundamentally disagree about something, both of which are great and for a scientist is

a delight. So keep it coming. And of course, when people agree and they agree and make it clear

that they agree from a stance of understanding, that of course is also gratifying. So it's exactly

what you're saying. And it's one of the upsides, I think, of social media, which is that unless

people block their comment section, and I do occasionally block people if they're being offensive

to other people. You say whatever you want to me. That's alienating people. That's not inviting

people into a conversation. That's not constructive. I actually have a rule, which is I call it

classroom rules. I've never announced it, but I allow for classroom rules. You can swear,

but you can't swear at people. That's what I was taught in graduate school.

Swear, but you can't swear at people. It's also a rule at home, although we try not to swear.

So you can swear, but swearing at people is not okay. And that a certain decorum is

required in order to have open discourse. So that works for me. I think that

it's been a while since I've been in school, but I work at a school. And I think that the ability to

not just reinforce, but challenge one's own stances, which sometimes leads to reinforcing

our own stances. It may. Well, man, that's legitimate. I mean, I have to assume that in

high schools, they still do debates and things of that sort. I mean, do they allow that? I mean,

throw kids in a class and say, let's debate something really controversial,

but you have to debate it from the other side. I mean, just as an experiment of forcing the brain

to try to be effective for sake of winning, but from the other perspective or stance, it seems

like a great exercise. If I were a high school teacher, that's the first thing I'd do. We pick

the most controversial topic, and then I'd ask people to divide along that topic,

and then I'd swap them into the other one and have them argue from the other one's stance.

Yep. Learning to appreciate perspectives is very.

And we'd use 14-ounce gloves. No, I'm kidding. It wouldn't be physical. It would be purely

intellectual. Yeah. I mean, can we take it back to the brain for a moment,

to the conversation that we were having earlier? So we were talking about that in our experiments

and now in whole bodies of neuroscientific knowledge, we know that there is this very

interesting neurobiological sort of processing difference between emotions and the thoughts

that are part of those emotions that are the result of those emotions that are also

incipitating those emotions. Like that whole process, when it pertains to the direct actions,

observable characteristics, behaviors of another person or situation that you can actually

pretty much directly learn or infer, as compared to when you have to bring a whole

lot of conceptual content knowledge to bear, experiential knowledge, simulation capacity

to bear, to be able to fully appreciate the nature of a situation. And we talked about how

that second kind of processing that I called transcendent, is the essence about distancing

yourself from the immediate physical situation, the observable, perceivable situation in a direct

sense, and instead constructing a narrative in your mind that's built from that, but that then

brings to bear all these other kinds of information that allow you to elaborate this into a narrative

that takes on emotional meaning and psychological power as a narrative. It becomes part of identity,

beliefs, all that kind of stuff. And we talked about that kind of thinking being associated

with the so-called default mode, which is deactivated systematically and decoupled from

itself. The different regions aren't talking to each other. When you are in the world acting,

doing a task, paying attention, inferring the direct things that you need to notice around

you, you're in the middle of playing a soccer game, the ball's coming at your head, that's not

a time to stop and muse about title nine and girls access to sports. You're going to trip

and fall, or you're going to miss your shot at the goal, or you're going to get hit with the ball.

We need to manage that space in order to have these conversations. I think what's important

here is to remember that the default mode network that is the substrate that is playing out your

own sense of self and inner consciousness and self-awareness, and is also the basis on which

we construct these broader inferential narratives that are the elaborative stuff of stories and

beliefs are fundamentally incompatible. The activation of those systems is fundamentally

incompatible with needing to be vigilant into the immediate physical or social situation around

you. If you feel physically, emotionally, culturally, socially unsafe, and you feel

that you need to watch your back, either literally or metaphorically, as you're thinking about things,

neurobiologically, that situation is incondusive. It is not conducive to being able to actually

conjure an alternative perspective in which you construct a meaningful narrative with

alternate ethical implications, with alternate prospective possible future outcomes, with

alternate views of the historical precedent or context. Being able to mentally time travel

into the space of those ideas is only really possible when people feel safe to think together.

It sounds like it's anti-creative. Yes. Creativity is also associated with the

activations of these networks, causally so in some recent work.

I had the good fortune of having dinner last year with somebody. I won't reveal who it is,

but he runs a major social media platform. He told me that in Japan, it's common for people to have

two or three or even as many as seven different social media handles, and that they

do this in order to embody different versions of themselves safely. These are not troll accounts.

These are not the accounts, and by the way, I see you troll accounts, that say whatever and then

you go to their accounts at some private account where they hide. Rather, these are individuals

who have multiple accounts. In one account, they might be a bit aggressive, maybe even a bully

online, dare I say. In another account, they might be very fawning and show up as the person

that everyone knows them to be in the real world. In another account, they might be a

university professor. In another, they're an athlete. It's fabricated in the sense that the

posts that they put up often don't accurately represent who they are in the real world,

but it's accurate in the sense that it represents the different dimensions of their persona that

are driving their real world decision-making at some level. It's pretend play for little kids.

It's pretend play, but it's not pretend because it's in cyberspace. I'll just go back to Rick Rubin

who, in addition to being this incredible music producer, is an enormous fan of professional

wrestling for many years. I asked him from a perplexity, I was like, why professional wrestling?

He said the athleticism. He said it's the only thing that's real because everyone agrees it's

not real. These are characters. You're agreeing for it to not be real, and yet it allows these

characters to fully embody these different personas. I had the experience years ago at

Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory summer camp for scientists where I attended and taught. I was

in a cab driving out to Cold Spring Harbor from the train station. I got into a discussion with

a cab driver and he said, okay, you're from California. He said, New York accent. I won't

try and imitate. He said, you're from California. He said, you're governor who at the time was

Schwarzenegger. He said, he's great. I said, tell me more. I happened to like Schwarzenegger for a

number of reasons. He actually signed my PhD because he was governor. I went to a UC. He said, well,

because if terrorists show up in California, he's going to go out there with a machine gun and take

them down. In his adult mind, he's the terminator. I realized in that moment, this was a smart guy,

this cab driver was a smart guy, that it wasn't a lack of narrative distancing. He had conflated

the actor with the roles he played. I realized in that moment that this was not a reflection of him

being unintelligent. It was a reflection of the fact that the brain often collapses identities

of others and makes these, I think it's just an efficient way to parse the world.

Yeah, we decide and then that's that kind of this person and we put them over there on a shelf.

So to return to the discussion that we're having, I think that the ability to embody different

aspects of self, but also the ability to transiently embody the personas of other people and to do

that in a way that allows for really thorough exploration of idea space, I feel like it can

only be a good thing provided it doesn't get physically violent or something. But that to me

seems like the exact opposite of what's happening now, which is that people are siloing off into

their camps where specific language and specific ideas are accepted and others are not. I mean,

it's so interesting and perplexing and disturbing to me that the way that certain

things that have nothing to do with politics get lumped with one group or the other, you know,

that it's so crazy to me on the one hand. And yet I think what you're describing seems to me

the route out of all of this. I really mean that. I feel like the education system starting young

and getting people emotionally engaged, learning what they like, what they don't like,

but then also teaching them about their emotional systems and how it helps them parse the world

is really the solution. So that when we're upset, we can realize like, yeah, I'm upset,

it makes sense why I'm upset. Let me explore it from the other side. It also makes sense why

they're upset. And that seems to be what humans have done somewhat throughout history,

never perfectly well. But it seems like it ought to be possible. I mean, the forebrain

is there for a reason. So could you in wanting to go back to a little bit of the biology and the

research, what have you seen in terms of cross cultural consistency about the role of emotions

in our ability to parse and learn? And because obviously we're not going to solve these problems

today. But although I think you shine light on some potential solutions, what do we know for

sure about human beings and their capacity to do what you're describing, to really learn differently?

It worked in the classroom where you were teaching, but how could each and every one of us do this?

I mean, how would we approach this? I guess I want to take this to the practical.

What can we do when we read a newspaper article? What can we do when we're on social media? What

can we do when our kid is like refusing to do something because they simply don't like it or

the teacher, they don't like the teacher. Are there paths through that that you've identified or

that you can sense work? I can get funny examples of my own kids when they didn't like things at

school. What tools do you use? So my son, when he was in third grade, he was very upset about

the behavior chart that his teacher had at school. So they had a behavior chart at the

back of the room that the principal didn't agree with this, but that teacher was there for a year.

So there was this behavior chart and you have green, you start on green with your little clip,

and then there's yellow, and then there's red, which is like call your parents, which I never

understood why they don't put call your parents on the green, but anyway. So you start on the green

and then you get down to the yellow and they get down to red, and there's Ted's little friend is

always getting on the red by 9 a.m. It's like, can we just get it over with? He tried to talk

with his teacher about why this behavior chart made him so uncomfortable. She could not understand

his perspective because she kept saying, but you're always on green. You're always doing what

you're supposed to be doing and you're respectful and you're well-behaved, so why is it a problem?

And what he was trying to say was that somehow it just made him uncomfortable to have that there.

So he was constantly bothering me with this. I finally told him I was trying to work one day

and he was home from school because I would let him work from home some days because we needed

to to kind of buffer a little bit. And you know, he'd bring all his work home and he'd do it himself.

I'd be working, he'd be working, right? It's fine. He had all kinds of projects going on,

you know. And this is a kid who, this is a little side story. This is a kid who went to first grade

and about two weeks into first grade, good first grade class. He was crying on a Sunday night to

me like, go to school. I don't want to go to school. I'm like, well, what's wrong with school?

I'm thinking he's getting bullied. Something's wrong with me. He's like, I don't know. He finally

looks at me and he goes, I have so much work to do. How do you expect me to get my work done

if I'm sitting in school all day? I can relate. I can relate. Can you relate? Because you're

actually a motivated, right? We take kids' motivations and the things they're interested

in and we sideline them and try to structure them into something. So back to this.

There are Legos to the build. Yeah. Oh, he was, he was way into building armor at that time. He

would, yeah, I know. We're probably terrible parents, but we gave him some safety glasses

and we taught him how to use it and we explained how metal is sharp and we gave him some shoe

metal and some tin snips. And he made a whole suit of armor in the backyard at, you know,

second grade. Anyway, it took him months and months. I mean, chain mail, the whole bit. He

was super into it. Amazing. You know, anyway, and he made airplane. He did all kinds of things.

But so here's this kid and he's bugging me about his teacher in this behavior chart. I said, Ted,

go write a letter to your teacher. If it bothers you that much, you go write a letter about why

it bothers you, right? Because in doing so, he's first of all helping to solve the problem.

Secondly, he was, he was formulating his understanding of what this behavior chart is

and why specifically it bothers him. And in so doing, it helps him not be so bothered by it,

right? So that's an example of something you could do, right? So he wrote this letter to his

teacher, which ended up being published in the National Academy of Science, Engineering and Maths

book, How People Learn, volume two, because I was on the committee of people that wrote it and we

needed an example of kids making sense out of motivational things and actually took his name

and the teacher often put the letter in the book. It basically is a little kid saying, listen,

teacher, when you put up this behavior chart, he called it a bad behavior chart, which it wasn't,

it was just a behavior chart, but he interprets it as a bad behavior chart. When you put that up,

it's as if you're, you're daring me to do something bad. You're, you're basically,

he doesn't say it like this, he says, you're basically making me uncomfortable because you are

laying out a perspective on me, a possibility space for me, that you're now bringing into the

conversation that I could be like that. And let's see if you're going to be, oh, not today,

oh, we're still angry, right? And so where does this go? It goes back to the idea that kids are,

and all of us are, interpreting the interactions and the structures around us, not only for what

they are, but for what they represent as somebody else's interpretation of what we are or are not

capable of. And he saw that behavior chart as a marker that his teacher assumed that all

kids in that class are capable of being badly behaved and that their main aim of being in school

is to be well behaved, right? And so he writes all about saying, saying, dear teacher, every day I

come to school, every single day, and every single day is new. That's what he says. And I could learn

something new, except then I see the bad behavior chart, right? He's saying school is supposed

to be about learning and us engaging, and you're making it about something so low level and basic

as are you going to behave yourself today? We are insulting him by the way we frame the context.

So take it back to the bigger issues of civic discourse and all these things. I think so much

of the way that we're organizing our lives, our social relationships, our community, our civic

structures right now is mirroring that teacher's behavior chart, right? Did she take the chart

down? I don't know. I don't think so. And I ask because I'm not sure that it matters. I think

what probably matters is that he had the chance to voice his understanding of it. His understanding

of the chart. Yeah, that's right. And now anybody can read his understanding of the chart because

it's published in the most widely read textbook on learning and motivation.

There's a couple of points. First is that the way we structure our environment can unwittingly

impose our mental models of other people's possibility spaces onto them and people find

that inherently abhorrent, right? So think about how we're doing that in many contexts,

not simply in schools. And then the second thing is from the Kent's perspective,

deconstructing exactly why and something bothers you by understanding how it is that you are

interpreting that thing, then opens you up to be able to manage those spaces in a new way and to

engage in them in a new way. So if we take the conversation back to the idea of civic discourse,

of civic reasoning, of engaging with any idea, right? There are ideas that are deeply problematic.

There are ideas that are deeply hurtful that have long histories of trauma associated with them,

of long histories of power dynamics and oppression associated with them. The way in which I think

we deconstruct those ideas is going to be critical to how those ideas live on implicitly

in our social relationships and our society. If we cancel them, if we negate them and pretend

they don't exist, all we're doing is burying them in a place where they can't be deconstructed and

only by actually taking them apart and appreciating the pain, the relationship structures, the limitations,

the resource allocations, the inequities that are implicit in those concepts only by deconstructing

and deeply understanding those, can we rebuild them in a different way? So it's very difficult

because on the one hand, we have a space created for ourselves right now in society that is deeply

unsafe for many people. And when you're in an unsafe space, you are not in a space that is

conducive to constructing and deconstructing meaning using those default mode systems and

other systems just to be crass about the brain and kind of oversimplify it, that are the substrate

of autobiographical self, of possibility spaces, of ethics, of deep moral and ethical emotions.

So on the one hand, we have a space that is deeply unsafe for individuals to think together and

genuinely so. There are real implications for people to reveal certain kinds of identities,

to engage with certain kinds of ideas in culturally formulated spaces that we've constructed

together. And the irony is that we can only fix that and create a different way of interacting

with one another by actually boldly going in there together. So it's a very nuanced

line where we need to develop skills, and this is where I think and many people think now,

that schools should be focused across disciplinary domains, whether it's math,

science, social studies, history, art, the arts, right? Sports should be focused on helping young

people and teachers develop capacities and dispositions for deconstructing and constructing

again, safe, safe cultural spaces to think together about, you know, interpretations,

about narratives, about stories, about assumptions, about ideas. Because as we engage

in those thoughts together, we call that civic discourse, right? We learn kind of rules for

not triggering and sensibilities for not endangering another person's ability to engage on

equal footing with us. Because if we trigger those unsafe, right, dangerous places for people,

they can't neurobiologically then engage with us deeply around sharing their perspective

and deconstructing ours together to build something where we have a shared understanding

in the middle. We have to trust one another and trust trusting one another really means we have to

have a space established in which we can feel safe to deconstruct our own beliefs and to allow

others to do the same and to assure them that we can engage with those beliefs no matter what they

are and then actually exteriorize them and evaluate them together and think about them around core

values we probably both hold, like well-being, like sustainability of society and of cultures

and of groups, right? These things are core. Everyone wants to be well. Everyone wants to have

a sustainable life and a life future and a cultural set of values. And so when we all

appreciate that we're bringing those things to the table but then are systematic about constructing

a space for civic discourse in which we are supporting one another in deconstructing our

own beliefs rather than each other's beliefs, right? Then we are at a space where we can start to

construct some kind of understanding, some kind of nuanced, more adaptive, more prosocial in the

true sense way of engaging with one another with not necessarily a way of agreeing with one another

but way of engaging and constructing and deconstructing meaning together so that we can

be adaptive, so that we can build a society where everyone can flourish, so that we can build a

society where everyone can belong and can actually have the resources they need.

I would argue as long as free speech is not possible for everybody, that nobody is safe.

Yes, that's right.

Then nobody is safe.

Nobody is safe.

And that there's an illusion of safety around the idea that people who have voice

are going to get what they want simply because they are the ones who are allowed to talk and

other people aren't. I mean, I think you said it perfectly when you said that anytime ideas get

buried, there's no way they can be solved. We know this from the scientific literature

for instance. There are results within social science and biological science that are

deeply troubling. I can think of experiments that were done in the realm of neurosurgery

on humans in the 1960s. People stimulating different brain areas and seeing rage or seeing

very politically controversial ideas emerge from the person's mouth in real time as a function of

stimulating that brain area. And then you say, well, did they really believe that and

they just never were saying it? And the person doesn't even recall that happening during the

surgery. I mean, this idea that Jung had that we have all things inside of us, I think can be seen

as a very dangerous notion and territory that we have all these shadows. But I'm also an optimist

and I feel that the optimistic view of it is that by knowing that we have all things inside of us

potentially and by embracing that fact that we can manage that to steal what you just said,

we can manage that and that we can function so much better when we see something in the world

that we think, that's not me. I'm not that and I hate that when if we understand that that also

lives inside of us, but that we just don't realize it. And I realize some people will hear this and

they'll go, that's not true. I have my stances and I disagree with other things. I would say,

absolutely yes. But the difference between one person's stance and another person's stance is

could be purely developmental wiring. It could be a difference of having read

different childhood books and oriented towards one book versus another. I think that we are

very similar at the level of core wiring and core algorithms that we run. But somehow these days,

we have the perception that we've diverged so much. I think the only thing that's really missing

is what you're describing is a place where any and all ideas can be explored freely,

not to establish consensus or validity of certain kinds of ideas, but to actually

exteriorize them and deconstruct them for what they actually are. Absolutely.

Thank you for working through that space because it's a tricky one.

It's very fraught. It's very fraught, but so very important. I have a question that's very basic,

but I've never gotten a good answer on. I was raised thinking that mirror neurons were a real

thing, that there are these neurons that exist in the brains of us and other old world primates,

like macaque monkeys, but especially in humans, the so-called mirror neurons that are activated

when we see somebody experience something and it evokes a sort of empathic understanding in us.

I've also seen some reviews written recently in some popular press saying that mirror neurons are

perhaps not playing the critical role that we thought they were. What's the story on mirror

neurons? We're not going after anybody's work in particular. I just want to know whether or not

there's real validity to this notion of mirror neurons.

I'm not an expert on it, but I can tell you what I know about it and the way that I think about it.

I think it's pretty clear now that there are no such things as mirror neurons, like some special

kind of cell type that's in the brain. They have not been found. They were predicted, but they were

not found. But something else was also predicted back in the late 1980s by Antonio Demasio,

where he talked about the brain being organized in terms of what he called convergent and

divergent zones. He talked about the brain being organized as networks converging and

then diverging again back out. You have places where processing is kind of coming together,

and then what happens in there then determines how things get spread back out. You've got these

sort of loops happening in the brain. His thinking on that was very much commensurate with others

thinking about the notion of goal-directed action and perception. If you think back to

developmental scholars who had knew nothing about the brain very much, like Jean Piaget back in the

early 20th century, where he was observing young children and noticing that they were

interacting with the world and they expected certain things. He thought imposing theories or

schemas onto the world and then accommodating was the word he used, the world with their actions,

when it didn't act the way they expected, and then assimilating that back to change what they

expected next time. So that he had this model that he built from systematically observing

children, three in particular, where what he realized is that kids are not just flailing

around discovering things haphazardly, they're imposing a certain logic onto the world, and then

they're systematically testing that logic. So they're hypothesis testing? Basically, yes. They're

expecting things, and then when the world does what they want, that reinforces, and when it does

something different, that's surprising, and then they have to accommodate and make sense, and then

they have to expect differently in the future. So what does this have to do with mirror and

irons? I think when you bring these different ideas together, the psychological observational

ideas and then the neurobiological ideas, what we basically have, and I wrote about this a little

bit in like, I think 2008, I have a paper called something like the smoke around mirror neurons,

and I forget the second half of the title, but it has the word goals and directed actions and things.

The idea, I think, is it's not that there are special neurons that are firing when we see

another person do the thing, but that we are, it goes back to the notion of us imposing our

expectations onto the world. You have to share and understand intuitively the goal of the other

one's action in order to activate these mirror regions, and what are those mirror regions?

They are basically regions that are deeply interconnected with each other. They're thoroughly

interconnected with each other in terms of white matter fiber tracks, and they are regions involved

in action planning, goal-oriented actions, and perceiving the outcomes of those actions.

So it's a kind of a loop between acting and perceiving and acting and perceiving, and I

argued at the time that goals are emergent, like high-level goals, are emergent from the dynamic

feedback loops of acting and perceiving. So I was really taking a very Piagetian view,

but imposing that on the neuroscience. So I think you take what I'm saying together with

like a Piagetian constructivist view, there are many other constructivist neuropsychologists

also, and then also the neural data. What we see is that we don't have these special neurons built

into our head. What we have is a natural proclivity, and I don't know where that comes from, but we

have a natural proclivity to try to appreciate another person's actions, feelings, experiences

by leveraging our own similar actions, feelings, experiences. And so when we can share goals or

experiences, that becomes more facile, and that's been shown over and over in these mirror-type

papers. And when you distance yourself from those goals and actions or don't have an intuitive

sense of them, then you don't get these mirroring activations, you don't get these kind of ramped

up sharing of goals or of experience. So I think it really comes back to the way the nervous system

is wired to be inherently social. We are cultural learners, we are situated in social spaces from

the moment we're conceived, and certainly from the moment we're born, and that social space,

observing others, interacting with others, co-regulating each other's physiology, each

other's attention, each other's emotion, right? As we do those things, we accommodate to each

other and we wire ourselves to expect certain kinds of feelings and then to recognize those same

things in other people. And so as we share constructed experience together, we start to

appreciate the sameness, right? The parallels between other people's and our own emotions,

thoughts, goals, and we can also dehumanize them, you know, make the other person not share our

thoughts, emotions, goals, and then we are capable of all kinds of horrible things we've talked about

before, right? Where you've actually distanced yourself. So what's the scope on mirror neurons?

I don't think mirror neurons exist, I think that's the consensus, but our propensity to engage with

other people by simulating on the substrate of our own self and then inferring the goals and the

feelings and the outcomes and the experiences of those experiences that we've simulated,

that's what is very essential to being a human. But keeping in mind that there's also this

layer of learned, lived, cultural, developed expectations we impose onto the world and we,

not filter, but we steer our attention, we steer our perception to accommodate,

to align with our expectations. So it's never just the reality of what the person experienced or

what happened, it's always our perception of that reality as we expected it to happen. So there's

this very dynamic cultural co-construction happening that is messy, that is iterative,

that you can learn to do in different ways, in different contexts, and that's kind of how I

understand this notion of mirroring. Before we conclude, I do want to answer your son's question.

So prior to recording, there was a text message that came, we don't have to read it verbatim,

but the text message, Mary Ellen's son is late teenage years and he's been doing

deliberate cold exposure, cold showers on a daily basis and reported that he's not get it,

he hasn't had any colds since starting this. This is actually a pretty common experience because

the pulse in adrenaline that is inevitable with a uncomfortably cold but safe.

Yeah. No, he jumps out of bed in the morning, does a whole bunch of exercises to get warm and

then jumps in a freezing cold shower. Amazing. That spike of adrenaline we know is neuro-protective.

If it's a short-lived spike in adrenaline, you don't want chronically.

No, you don't want chronic stress. That's not good for you.

That's not good. We know that from the beautiful work of Bruce McEwan and Bob Sapolsky and others.

Gorgeous work, yeah. But then he asked, should he get sick?

Should he continue the cold showers? And the answer is no. I think that then it would be

hot showers and hot baths and sauna type stuff is probably better, but not so hot that it's

stressful. You really want to reduce stress on an ill system. So he sounds, for many reasons,

like a remarkable young man. As is your daughter, it sounds like a remarkable.

And you're remarkable. I really mean that. I feel like we could go on forever exploring these

ideas. I absolutely would love to have you back for another discussion or many about your research.

I want to thank you for taking the time of your research schedule, your teaching schedule to come

educate us today. These ideas are so vitally important and you provide so many real world

examples. In fact, it's one of the things that I love so much about your work is that it's really

nested in real world applications and your thoughts and perspectives on the education

and how it could be better at the level of educating kids at home, teaching ourselves,

teachers and the education system. I hope will ring far and wide because they really

can be implemented. We're not talking about the need to purchase a bunch of stuff.

No, we need to start with a different disposition. We need to start with a different goal.

Yeah. The goal of education needs to not learning is not the goal. It's not the outcome. It needs

to be the development of the person, right? How is a person changing themselves having learned

this? Then you design the learning opportunities to change who people are capable of becoming.

The learning is there, but it's not the end point. It's just the means to something else,

which we haven't been attending to enough. That's the development of the person,

who they become having learned that. Beautifully put. Well, thank you so much for your time.

Thank you so much for the work you do. I can't wait to have another discussion with you about

the emerging research. Great. I'll be back. Thank you. Thank you for joining me today for

my discussion about emotions, social interactions and learning with Dr. Mary Helen Immordino-Yang.

I hope you found the conversation to be as informative and enriching as I did. If you'd

like to learn more about Dr. Immordino-Yang's research, please find the link to her laboratory

website in the show note captions. In addition, Dr. Immordino-Yang authored an incredible book

called Emotions, Learning and the Brain. It's a book designed for the general public.

It's incredibly informative and has a lot of practical tools as well. We've provided a link

to that book in the show note captions. If you're learning from and or enjoying this podcast,

please subscribe to our YouTube channel. That's a terrific zero cost way to support us. In addition,

please subscribe to the podcast on both Spotify and Apple. And on both Spotify and Apple,

you can leave us up to a five star review. If you have questions for me or comments about

the podcast or guests that you'd like me to include on the Huberman Lab podcast,

please put those in the comment section on YouTube. I do read all the comments.

Please also check out the sponsors mentioned at the beginning and throughout today's episode.

That's the best way to support this podcast. Not on today's episode, but on many previous

episodes of the Huberman Lab podcast, we discuss supplements. While supplements aren't necessary

for everybody, many people derive tremendous benefit from them for things like enhancing sleep,

for hormone support and for focus. The Huberman Lab podcast is proud to have partnered with

Momentus Supplements. To see the supplements discussed on the Huberman Lab podcast,

go to livemomentus spelled OUS. So that's livemomentus.com slash Huberman. Again,

that's livemomentus.com slash Huberman. If you're not already following us on social media,

I am Huberman Lab on Instagram, Twitter, Facebook and LinkedIn. And at all those places,

I discuss science and science related tools, some of which overlaps with the content of the

Huberman Lab podcast, but much of which often does not overlap with the content of the Huberman

Lab podcast. So again, it's Huberman Lab on all social media platforms. In addition,

if you haven't subscribed to our neural network newsletter, it's a zero cost monthly newsletter

that provides summaries of podcast episodes as well as toolkits. For instance, toolkits for

optimizing sleep or toolkits for learning and neuroplasticity or for deliberate cold exposure

or for dopamine and on and on. To sign up for the neural network newsletter, simply go to

hubermanlab.com, go to the menu, scroll down to newsletter and provide your email. We do not share

your email with anybody. Thank you once again for joining me for today's discussion with Dr.

Mary Helen Imordino Yang. And last but certainly not least, thank you for your interest in science.

Machine-generated transcript that may contain inaccuracies.

In this episode, my guest is Mary Helen Immordino-Yang, EdD, professor of education, psychology and neuroscience at the University of Southern California and director of the Center for Affective Neuroscience, Development, Learning and Education, who has done groundbreaking research on emotions, self-awareness and social interactions and how these impact the way we learn and change across our lifespan. She explains how an understanding of emotions can be leveraged to improve learning in children and in adults, and how the education system should be altered to include new forms of exploration and to facilitate better learning and to include more diverse learning (and teaching) styles. This episode ought to be of interest to anyone interested in how we learn, human development in children and adults, as well as those generally interested in education, psychology or neuroscience.
For the full show notes, visit hubermanlab.com.
Thank you to our sponsors
AG1: https://athleticgreens.com/huberman
Eight Sleep: https://eightsleep.com/huberman
HVMN: https://hvmn.com/huberman
ROKA: https://roka.com/huberman
InsideTracker: https://insidetracker.com/huberman
Momentous: https://livemomentous.com/huberman
The Brain Body Contract
https://hubermanlab.com/tour
Timestamps
(00:00:00) Dr. Mary Helen Immordino-Yang
(00:02:11) Sponsors: Eight Sleep, HVMN, ROKA
(00:05:54) Inspiration, Awe & Story
(00:09:59) Brain-Body, Narratives
(00:15:58) Emotions, Durability & Lifespan
(00:21:47) Conjuring Stories, Historical Context & Emotion
(00:32:16) Sponsor: AG1
(00:33:30) Hierarchal Emotion Organization, Default Mode Network, Story & Emotion
(00:46:24) Emotional Development & Lifetime
(00:57:13) Narrative & Genocide; Checking Assumptions & Mental Flexibility
(01:05:22) Social Media, Cognitive Dissonance
(01:09:52) Education, Deconstructing Beliefs & Curiosity
(01:17:22) Sponsor: InsideTracker
(01:18:32) Emotion & Learning; Constructing Meaning
(01:28:59) Good Teachers & Curiosity
(01:33:25) Inter-disciplinary Education; Development & Culture
(01:50:58) Idea Exploration, Tolerance
(01:56:53) Reframing Education, Deconstructing Assumptions
(02:03:28) Safety, Creativity & Default Mode Network
(02:12:15) Civic Discourse & Education; Deconstructing Ideas
(02:27:31) “Mirror” Neurons, Shared Social Experiences
(02:35:49) Cold Exposure & Sickness; Role of Education
(02:38:51) Zero-Cost Support, YouTube Feedback, Spotify & Apple Reviews, Sponsors, Momentous, Social Media, Neural Network Newsletter
Title Card Photo Credit: Mike Blabac
Disclaimer