Huberman Lab: Dr. David Linden: Life, Death & the Neuroscience of Your Unique Experience

Scicomm Media Scicomm Media 8/21/23 - Episode Page - 2h 37m - PDF Transcript

Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast, where we discuss science and science-based tools

for everyday life.

I'm Andrew Huberman, and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology at Stanford

School of Medicine.

Today, my guest is Dr. David Linden.

Dr. David Linden is a professor of neuroscience at Johns Hopkins School of Medicine.

His laboratory has studied neuroplasticity, that is, how connections in the brain change

in response to experience.

Much of that work focused on a structure called the cerebellum, which is also sometimes

referred to as the mini-brain, because it looks like a mini-brain in the bottom and

back of the human brain, and it's responsible for an enormous number of basic functions

that we use in everyday life, including our motor behavior, that is, our ability to walk

and talk, but also dance, play, instruments, and it's responsible for an enormous number

of basic functions that we use in everyday life, including our sense of balance, our

ability to learn new motor behaviors, as well as our sense of timing.

Today we will discuss the cerebellum and what it does, but Dr. David Linden will also teach

us about the important sense of touch, as well as what makes us different as individuals.

The reason today's discussion encompasses so many important topics is that Dr. David

Linden's laboratory has focused on many of those topics, and he is also the author of

five excellent popular books about neuroscience that focus on, for instance, our sense of

pleasure and where it originates from and what controls it in the brain, as well as

our sense of touch.

And today we start off our discussion by talking about the recent discovery of a set of neurons

that have been known about for a long period of time, but that only recently have been

characterized that are involved in sensual touch in particular, and it's a fascinating

conversation, I assure you.

In addition to that, Dr. David Linden informs us about what makes us individuals, how each

and every one of us perceives the same things differently.

And it's an absolutely fascinating conversation, which tells you, for instance, why some of

you think a smell is putrid, indeed, smells like a vomit, whereas others perhaps are not

bothered by that smell and why others still are attracted to that smell or something that

you look at or something that you hear.

We also talk about nature versus nurture and how we come to be who we are, not just through

our genes and epigenetics, but also through our early childhood experience and adult experience.

And then in the latter third of our conversation, we shift to talking about the so-called mind-body

connection and the science underlying how our thoughts inform our bodily health or lack

thereof, as well as how the organs of our body control the chemicals, hormones, and

thoughts within our brain.

Then we shift to discussing Dr. David Linden himself and the fact that in 2020, he was

diagnosed with a form of heart cancer that led his physicians to tell him that he had

six to 12 months to live.

Now, obviously, because he was in our studio to record this conversation, he has outlived

that prognosis, but he lives day to day with the knowledge that his death may very well

come soon, although it isn't clear exactly when that day will come, of course.

He tells us how the initial prognosis of his cancer, as well as outliving that prognosis,

has informed his day-to-day life, as well as his thinking and his relationships.

And that leads to a very direct and, frankly, emotional conversation that includes advice

on how all of us can get the most out of our daily living and out of our overall life.

It's an extremely powerful conversation that I believe everyone, regardless of age or health

status, can benefit from.

And it's one that makes clear that not only is Dr. David Linden a spectacular scientist,

but also a spectacular educator, a spectacular popular writer, a spectacular family man,

including husband and father and friend to many people and his colleagues.

But he is also a courageous and spectacularly generous human being.

Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast is separate from my teaching

and research roles at Stanford.

It is, however, part of my desire and effort to bring zero cost to consumer information

about science and science-related tools to the general public.

In keeping with that theme, I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast.

Our first sponsor is Roka.

Roka makes eyeglasses and sunglasses that are the absolute highest quality.

The company was founded by two All-American swimmers from Stanford, and everything about

Roka eyeglasses and sunglasses were designed with performance in mind.

I've spent a lifetime working on the biology of the visual system, and I can tell you that

your visual system has to contend with an enormous number of challenges in order for

you to be able to see clearly.

Roka understands those challenges and the biology of the visual system such that they've

designed sunglasses and eyeglasses that always allow you to see with crystal clarity.

Now, initially, Roka eyeglasses and sunglasses were designed for sports performance, and

as a consequence, all of their glasses are designed to be very lightweight and to not

slip off your face if you get sweaty.

However, the design of the glasses includes some that are specifically for sport and others

whose aesthetic really allows you to use them for sport as well as out to dinner or to work,

etc.

And that's how I use them.

If you'd like to try Roka eyeglasses and sunglasses, you can go to Roka.com.

That's R-O-K-A.com and enter the code Huberman to save 20% off your first order.

Again, that's Roka R-O-K-A.com and enter the code Huberman at checkout.

Today's episode is also brought to us by Levels.

Levels is a program that lets you see how different foods and behaviors affect your

health by giving you real-time feedback on your diet using a continuous glucose monitor.

One of the most important factors impacting your immediate and long-term health is the

way that your body manages its blood glucose or sometimes referred to as blood sugar levels.

To maintain energy and focus throughout the day, you want to keep your blood glucose steady

without big spikes or dips.

Using levels, you can monitor how different types of foods and different food combinations

as well as food timing and things like exercise combine to impact your blood glucose levels.

I started using levels a little over a year ago and it gave me a lot of insight into how

specific foods were spiking my blood sugar and then leaving me feeling tired for several

hours afterwards, as well as how the spacing of exercise and my meals was impacting my

overall energy.

And in doing so, it really allowed me to optimize how I eat, what I eat, when I exercise, and

so on, such that my blood glucose levels and energy levels are stable throughout the day.

If you're interested in learning more about levels and trying a continuous glucose monitor

yourself, go to levels.link.huberman.

Right now, Levels is offering an additional two free months of membership.

Again, that's levels.link, L-I-N-K, slash Huberman to get two free months of membership.

We are always striving to make the Huberman Lab podcast better and to that end, we need

your help.

Over the next month, we are going to be carrying out a survey.

The purpose of the survey is to improve the Huberman Lab podcast according to your feedback.

We put together a brief survey to understand what you love about the podcast, hopefully

you love a few things at least, or maybe just one thing, as well as what you think could

be improved, or perhaps the many things that you think could be improved about the Huberman

Lab podcast.

Basically, what we are asking is to get your feedback so that we can improve any and all

things about the Huberman Lab podcast.

The survey does not take long, and every single response will be reviewed.

As a thank you for completing the survey, we are offering two months free of the Huberman

Lab premium channel.

If you're already a member of the Huberman Lab premium channel, do not worry.

You will get an additional two free months for carrying out this survey.

You can find the link to the survey in the show notes for this podcast episode and on

our website, hubermanlab.com.

So if you would be so kind as to take a few minutes to fill out the survey and help us

continue with bringing you the best possible content here at the Huberman Lab podcast.

And as always, thank you for your interest in science.

And now for my discussion with Dr. David Linden.

Professor Linden, welcome.

Thanks so much for having me.

I've been looking forward to our conversation, and we have a lot to talk about.

We do.

Lots of different aspects of science, lots of different aspects of personal journey and

what you're confronting now as it relates to your health and your future.

I want to start off with a question that I learned from the one and only, the great

Carl Diceroth, who was the first guest on this podcast, my colleague at Stanford.

And for those of you that don't recognize Carl's name, he is a absolute phenom.

He's a active clinical psychiatrist.

So he's an MD and he also is a bioengineer who's developed a lot of the modern tools

for probing the brain.

And anytime I've met with Carl, the first thing he says is, what are you most excited

about lately?

That's a good question.

It is.

So I'm going to steal that approach and say, what are you most excited about lately?

Well, very, very lately, the most interesting thing that I read in neuroscience is the answer

to a scientific problem that I think is really dear to a lot of people's hearts, and that

is, what are the nerve endings in the genitals that are responsible for sexual sensation?

If you think about it, people can feel sexy from being touched on lots of different parts

of the body, but there's something special about the genitals.

Doesn't matter male or female or intersex or gay or straight or bi or whatever you are,

the genitals are a hotspot.

And why?

And you'd think as biologists, we'd know this by now.

This would be something we could just answer.

But it's been a mystery for a long time, and if you go back to 1860, there was a German

neuroanatomist named Krauss, and he cut thin sections of tissue from the penis and the clitoris,

and he looked at them under the microscope, and he saw a particular kind of nerve ending

there that has since been called the Krauss corpuscle.

And there were lots of them in these two places, and so he thought, well, maybe this is the

cellular basis of sexual sensation.

Maybe these are the particular nerve endings that are responsible for this.

But there were some things that were in favor of that and some not.

So these nerve endings are also in some other places that people can find more or less sexy,

like they're in the nipples, and they're in the lips, and they're in the anus, all places

that get popular in that domain.

But they're also in places like the cornea or the lining of the joints.

So distribution doesn't quite make sense, and so it was never known.

And so if you wanted to really test as a scientist whether these nerve endings are responsible,

you want to record their electrical signals while the genitals are being touched.

You'd want to inactivate these cells and see if you could interfere with sexual sensation.

And this, in a preprint from David Ginty's group at Harvard, is just what they had been

able to do in mice.

They found a way to label and record from and activate and inactivate artificially the

Krauss corpuscles.

And so you see a nerve ending in the skin.

It could be conveying all kinds of information.

It could be tuned for hot, or for cold, or for itch, or for pain, or for inflammation,

or for mechanical sensations stretching vibration indentation.

And sure enough, when they recorded from these Krauss corpuscles, they really are mechanical

sensors, as you would expect if they were involved in sexual sensation.

So that was good.

And then the other thing, then what they did is they tried to artificially turn them on.

And so the way they did that is they used genetic tricks to express one of Karl Dysaroth's

molecules that activates neurons when blue light is shown on them.

And they found that if they express this artificial protein in the Krauss cells in a male mouse

and then shine blue light, the mouse gets an erection.

All right, so far so good.

What happens if you turn them off?

Well, if you turn them off in a male mouse, it's just as interested in females when they're

in heat, but it won't mount and thrust and ejaculate as much.

And if you turn them off in a female mouse during the time of her cycle, where she would

normally be sexually receptive, you find that she is much less interested.

She's much less likely to let him mount.

She's much less likely to let him finish.

So this is the remarkable result that finally, after all these years, since 1860, now we

know what the nerve endings are that convey sexual sensation.

Like all good science, then there are a lot of questions that are really interesting to

our everyday lives.

People like different things in bed and have different propensity for orgasm or like to

be touched in different ways.

Well, is part of that reason because of individual variation in their Krauss corpuscle structure?

We know that sexual sensation diminishes with aging.

Is that in part because Krauss corpuscle density is lost from the skin of the genitals?

And that's a reasonable idea because we know, for example, that fine touch sensors in the

fingertips, so-called Merkel and Meisner endings, also named after German anatomists like so

many things are, are also lost with age.

So that's a reasonable idea.

So this finding from Ginti's lab has opened up a whole world of science.

And I've been, my own lab doesn't work on touch, but I've been a fanboy of touch for

many, many years, mostly because where I work at Johns Hopkins Medical School, there have

been many terrific touch researchers.

It's been a world center for it.

And I hear about it over lunch and I got all fired up.

So years ago, I wrote a book about it and I still follow the field.

And this is the most interesting thing in that field recently.

And as I recall, Ginti was your neighbor at Hopkins before he moved to Harvard.

That's right.

That's right.

He was one of the ones.

Ginti, Steven, Shao, Michael, Katerina, Xinjiang, Dong.

There have been a number of world leaders in the cellular basis of touch sensation at Hopkins.

Do you recall if in the preprint that you were describing, there was an experiment where

they activated these crouse corpuscles in females?

It's funny.

You should mention that.

I sent that exact email to David Ginti and they said they are in the process of doing

that right now and they don't quite know yet.

And so I asked him, I said, so, for example, is erection of the clitoris even a thing in mice?

And he says, well, we're really not sure.

So we're activating the crouse corpuscles in female mice and we're just kind of staring

at it and looking and see if anything happens.

Does the body change shape?

Is there a color change?

They don't even quite know what it is they're looking for because it's that much on the

leading edge of things.

But it's a good question.

Or perhaps the female mice would be more willing to mate outside of the usual time frame of

receptivity if these crouse corpuscles are stimulated.

That's possible.

My suggestion, my guess would be not, because I think that the hormonal regulation of

receptivity is like a sledgehammer and very hard to overcome.

But they might be more willing to continue mating or make for longer during their fertile time.

And I just want to remind people because we had a guest recently, Dr.

Rena Malik, who's a urologist, reproductive and sexual health expert.

She's an MD and she made clear that the clitoris and the penis come from the same

embryonic origin.

They are analogous tissues in different individuals.

I do have one more question about this sexual touch thing.

These are peripheral nerves, right?

So these are not of the brain and spinal cord.

They are in what we call the periphery.

And my understanding is that peripheral neurons regenerate and can remodel themselves

extensively in ways that neurons within the brain and spinal cord tend to remodel less,

especially as one gets older out of the so-called critical period.

Is it possible that these crouse corpuscles and their patterns of

innervation within the genitals change according to the stimulation that people experience?

In other words, is sexual sensation experience dependent?

That is a great question.

And so we don't know because monitoring this in people is not technically possible, right?

It requires cadaver to shoot, so you can only do it once.

In animals, it will be possible and it could be for a couple of different reasons.

In other words, I think what you're imagining is that there's actual structural plasticity.

If you looked at these crouse corpuscles, that you would actually see them changing

their shape or their size or their density as a result of experience.

But what can also happen is a phenomenal desensitization.

That is to say, when there's stimulation for a long time, then the receptors

transiently can become less sensitive to touch.

And it's well known, particularly in males, that chronic masturbation can produce

desensitization of sexual sensation in the penis.

And that could be as a result of a physical change, a morphological change in the crouse

corpuscles, but it's more likely to be a change in their function that you wouldn't be able to

simply see by looking at an outline of their structure in the microscope.

Such an interesting topic.

Thanks for opening things up with that.

And I'll have to check out this preprint.

I'm also a huge fan of David Guindy's work and colleagues.

There are many people involved in that domain of work, of course.

I'd like to talk about your recent book and the sort of underlying basis of what led you to write it.

And what intrigued you about this idea of human individuality?

The book, Unique, is one that we'll provide a link to in the show note captions.

And it's a very interesting idea that we are all different, especially coming from a neuroscientist

who were trained at least similarly to learn that sure, the bumps and ripples of the brain

and the fine wiring of the brain is different.

And we are all unique and different.

We have different shapes, aka morphologies, but focusing on human individuality is not something

that modern neuroscience or classic neuroscience has really done much of.

It's really focused on how people do X or people do Y this way.

Tell us about Unique and tell us about human individuality.

Yeah, well, I mean, you're absolutely right.

So when I look at the experiments in my own lab, how do we do them?

Well, we work on mice.

Do we work on mice with genetic variation?

No, we work on highly inbred mice that are designed to be as genetically similar

to each other as possible.

And then we raise them basically in prison in little cells, which may not be a good idea.

And we try to give them as similar experience as possible.

They are given toys and food and water, but I agree, it resembles a prison of sorts.

They aren't free to roam.

They have nothing like the experience of a wild mouse.

Let me put it that way.

And yes, as you said correctly, there are plenty of experiments where there is enrichment

for mice, and they love it.

So for example, in our lab, when we put running wheels in the cages or mice and let them run

overnight, they're active at night, your average mouse will run two kilometers in a night for

a little tiny mouse.

And some of the mice are so intense, they will run 20 kilometers.

Imagine a mouse doing 20K, but it will happen.

They really, really like it.

They don't like being in prison.

They want to exercise, and they're really bored.

So yes, to get back to your general point, so much of science is designed to try to find

general principles of function of the brain, of physiology, of genetics,

and to ignore individual variation.

But individual variation is so important to our human experience, and actually is so

important to the process of evolution and natural selection and how species make their way

in the world, that it's something that requires a lot of attention.

And to me, what's really fascinating is that when you look at the variation in the way sense

organs function, it's almost a miracle that we can agree on a common reality at all,

even within the human species.

And this is true of some senses more than others.

Obviously, in your world, in the retina, we have various kinds of loss of color vision

that are well known, and some other more complicated phenomena having to do with

impairments in the perception of motion or form.

But the place where this really happens is in the olfactory system.

So we have approximately 400 functional receptors for different odorant molecule smells

in our nose, and if you sequence the genomes of many people, you find that the DNA that encodes

for these odorant receptors is unusually variable from individual to individual.

As a matter of fact, if you take two different people, on average, they will have functional

differences in 30% of their odor receptors.

And if you do as Leslie Vossall and her colleagues did at Rockefeller University and give odor tests

where they give people different things to smell and then they dilute them and find the

threshold at which they can detect them, you find enormous changes from people to people.

Both in general terms, some people are just better smellers than others,

but in terms of individual odors as well.

There are some odors that some people can't detect and other people smell one way.

For example, there is a secreted hormone called androstinone.

Androstinone, there are some people who can't smell at all.

For some people, it smells rather pleasant, like cut grass.

And for some people, it smells foul, like urine or sweat.

And it just depends on genetic variation in one particular odorant receptor.

Sorry to interrupt.

Another phenomenal researcher who studies olfaction, among other things, Catherine Doolock,

I once heard say that some people have a gene that for them makes the smell of microwave popcorn.

They experience that smell as vomit.

And other people who lack this gene like the smell of microwave popcorn,

or at least for them, it's not aversive.

So it can really be a binary response.

Well, it can.

And actually, that's a very particular funny case.

So the relevant chemical there is butaric acid and also isovelaric acid.

And so there are researchers, I think Rachel Hertz is one of them,

who have given a mixture of these two chemicals to people.

And if they say, this is parmesan cheese, they go, oh, yeah, that's parmesan cheese.

And if they give it to other people and say, this is vomit, they'll go, oh, yeah, that's vomit.

And if they tell people, they give them one vial and say, this is parmesan cheese.

And they go, yeah, and they give them another one.

They say, it's vomit.

And they go, yeah.

And then they say, well, actually, we fooled you.

It was the same vial.

They say, no, you didn't.

You must have made a mistake.

They're convinced that they couldn't have been the same thing.

So this points out, not only is there genetic variation that is responsible for our individuals'

perceived odor, but we are incredibly suggestible in terms of odors.

And we are very dependent upon them in terms of cultural context.

And this can be learned.

And this is central to our humanity in the sense that we humans are what I like to call the anti-Pandas.

Pandas live in one spot in southern China, and they eat one thing, bamboo.

And that's it.

Humans are the opposite.

Humans can live in any ecological niche in the world from the tropics to the poles,

and humans eat a wide, wide, wide variety of foods.

And as a result, it means that we have to have a very plastic olfactory system.

There have to be very few things that we find innately aversive.

There are only a handful of odors, rotting meat odors.

Molecules with the evocative names like cadaverine and putrescene are things that even babies

when they're newborn find aversive.

But other things happen that they need to be learned.

For example, pretty much every adult finds poop odors unpleasant, but babies happily play with

their own poop.

They have to learn that that's disgusting.

It's not because babies have a different nose.

It's because they have to learn culturally.

It's not innate.

It is not innate.

There are only a few innate odor aversions and a few innate taste aversions that we're

born with, and the other things are elaborated culturally.

And we can think about this in terms of how we talk about odors.

So, for example, we might say vanilla smells sweet.

Well, that's weird.

That's like, those are two different senses.

How can something smell sweet?

That's like saying it sounds red, right?

It's like a statement about synesthesia.

Right, but how did it come to pass?

And do people say that vanilla smells sweet everywhere in the world?

Well, the answer is no.

So, in places in the world where vanilla is used with sugar in sweet foods like desserts,

then people say that vanilla smells sweet or mint similarly smells sweet if it is typically

used together with sugar.

But if you go to a place like Vietnam where mint is mostly used in savory dishes,

people won't say that mint smells sweet.

So, there's a paired association there that at least at our level of

conscious understanding feeds back on to what we call olfactory or smell perception.

But really, it must be a paired association at some point in development.

Yeah, it is.

It absolutely is a paired association and it's something that goes on continually

through your life, right?

I mean, lots of people, for example, have stories of foods that they wouldn't eat as a child,

but they came to like as an adult.

A good example of that is coffee.

A lot of people have to overcome bitter aversion to become coffee aficionados.

So, this feeds into the more general theme that there is no pure perception.

Perception is inference.

It's not like there is a purely objective world that can somehow make its way through

the senses and we can perceive that as the truth.

All of our perception through all of our senses, both the outward pointing senses of the world,

like smell and taste and sight and hearing, and the inward pointing senses like balance

and is my stomach full and things like that, all of them are based on experience

and expectation and the situation of the moment.

As many of you know, I've been taking AG1 daily since 2012,

so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring the podcast.

AG1 is a vitamin mineral probiotic drink that's designed to meet all of your foundational

nutrition needs.

Now, of course, I try to get enough servings of vitamins and minerals through whole food sources

that include vegetables and fruits every day, but oftentimes I simply can't get enough servings.

But with AG1, I'm sure to get enough vitamins and minerals and the probiotics that I need,

and it also contains adaptogens to help buffer stress.

Simply put, I always feel better when I take AG1.

I have more focus and energy and I sleep better and it also happens to taste great.

For all these reasons, whenever I'm asked if you could take just one supplement,

what would it be, I answer AG1.

If you'd like to try AG1, go to drinkag1.com slash huberman to claim a special offer.

They'll give you five free travel packs plus a year supply of vitamin D3K2.

Again, that's drinkag1.com slash huberman.

Are there any examples of uniqueness of visual perception that come to mind?

I recently did a social media post that involved, it was essentially three rings, a blue ring,

a red ring and a blue ring in the center.

Perhaps it was the other way around.

Excuse me, it was red, blue, red.

And I asked which ring is in front or are they all in the same plane?

Now, of course, it's a two-dimensional image and interestingly, it splits out into about

thirds.

Some people see the blue ring in front quite a bit, others see a red ring out in front,

others see them all in the same plane.

And this, we think, has to do with differences in two things between individuals.

One is the distribution of the cone photoreceptors, which we know is essentially random between

individuals, maybe even between the two eyes, and that gives rise to this phenomenon of chromatic

aberration, which is the displacement of the visual image according to the wavelength of

the light and we won't get into the physics of it now.

I'll soon do a post that hopefully distills it in a manner that's simple enough that people

understand.

But clearly, some people see certain colors in front of others and the person right next

to them could see the opposite color in front and others say, what are you talking about?

All the colors are in exactly the same plane of vision.

So that's the one that I know.

I'm guessing you know some others and perhaps some more robust ones.

Well, I think perhaps this is maybe not what you had in mind, but one way in which experience

modifies the visual world has to do with how much light you're exposed to in the first five years

or so of your life.

And so kids that don't get outside are much more likely to be myopic.

And it actually is near-sighted when they grow up than kids who got outside.

And we now know that at least part of the story is that light seems to stimulate the

expression of a class of molecules called trophic factors that you're well acquainted with

that actually change the shape of the eyeball.

So it's not really the structure of the retina or the lens of the cornea.

The actual degree of elongation of the eyeball changes changing the way the retina sits relative

to the lens and that seems to be light dependent early in life and which gives rise to a higher

incidence of myopia.

And to me, this is really, well, first of all, it's news you can use.

You should get your kids outside for all kinds of reasons.

And you should get outside too, especially in the morning, set that circadian rhythm.

I know that's a famous Hubert Minesk point.

They're going to be putting me in the grave, David, and I'm going to be telling people to,

or maybe it'll be on my tombstone.

He'll say, get sunlight in your eyes, especially on cloudy days because there is still sunlight,

even if you can't see the physical object of the sun, on cloudy days.

Well, I think this whole idea of having traits that are dependent upon early life experience

is fascinating because there are a number of situations where you would guess that something

is genetic, but it isn't.

It's actually dependent on early life experience.

And there's an amazing story about this having to do with the early days of World War II.

So in the early days of World War II, the Japanese army just swept through Asia.

They defeated the British and Malaysia and Singapore.

They overran Thailand and Burma, and they were knocking on the gates of India,

and everything was going great, except the Japanese army had a problem.

There were an enormous number of their soldiers who became incapacitated with heat stroke.

They got, their core temperature got too hot.

And when the army doctors examined them, they found this was much more likely to happen

in soldiers who came from the northern part of Japan, Hokkaido, where it snows in the winter,

as opposed to the southern part of Japan, like Kyushu, which is a semi-tropical environment.

And the classical explanation, the biologists like us would guess, would say, oh, all right,

well, this has happened genetically over many years.

You have a family that's been in Kyushu for many generations,

and you've selected for gene variants to allow you to tolerate the heat better.

And we know actually what this is.

So if you're more heat tolerant, it's because you have more of a particular class of sweat

gland called the acrine sweat glands, the sort of saltwater sweat glands, not the

lippity, stinky armpit sweat glands called the apocrine ones, the acrine ones.

You have a higher fraction of them that are innervated, meaning that the signals from your

brain that say your core is too hot can then make you sweat.

So the total density of sweat glands between northern and southern soldiers in Japan wasn't

different, but the southern soldiers tend to have a higher degree of innervation.

All right, so you say, okay, well, this happened genetically over many generations.

But if you look at those rare cases where you have soldiers from a long-established northern

family and their parents moved south and they grew up in the southern location,

they had high sweat gland innervation. They were well heat tolerant.

Conversely, if you had a well-established southern Kyushu family and they moved to

Hokkaido and then had their child, that child developed the northern sweat gland innervation

pattern. So meaning less nerve innervation of those sweat glands, as you mentioned before,

just as many sweat glands, just less nerve innervation. Therefore,

those sweat glands could not be activated. They couldn't dump heat as well. Their heat

tolerance was lower. Correct. Exactly right. And what's wonderful is that this

gives an advantage that you can't get through evolution and that it can happen right away in

one generation, right? Evolutionary change is slow, right? And you can adapt as a species and as a

family over many, many, many generations. But when you have a phenomenon that is set by

early life experience, well, then you can benefit from that early life experience

within your own life. It's not that your great, great, great, great, great grandchildren will

ultimately benefit. You benefit. Another wonderful example of this

comes from Fieldmice, Vols. And we were talking earlier about how we both worked with the

scientist Irv Zucker at Berkeley, who was a specialist in Vols. And what people found

is that if you take a wild caught Fieldmice and you have pregnant mothers and you have them in the

lab, but you manipulate the lights so that you have artificial spring. In other words,

day length is getting longer day after day during the pregnancy. Then what happens is

when their pups are born, they will have a low density of fur anticipating summer temperatures.

If you, however, put them in artificial fall where day length is getting shorter,

they will be born now with high density of fur anticipating winter temperatures. And of course,

you can do this no matter what the season actually is in the world by manipulating these

lights in the lab. And so like the sweating Japanese soldiers, this is a great example of

early life plasticity and just the sort of trait that if you ask someone, they would probably guess

is heritable, but actually is not. Thanks for mentioning Irv Zucker, who as you also mentioned

was an advisor to us both who's done incredible work in circadian biology, seasonal rhythms,

hormones and behavior. I have such reverence for Irv. And the experiment you mentioned made me

smile wide because it's but one of gosh, maybe hundreds of incredible studies. So if people

are interested in seasonal rhythms and circadian rhythms and biology of the most interesting

kind, definitely check out Irv Zucker's work at Berkeley. I'll provide a link to his PubMed there.

Since we've been taking a tour of individual variation in olfactory perception, visual perception,

and now heat tolerance, I have to ask, are you aware of any examples off the top of your head

in the auditory domain that particularly intrigued you?

Yeah, well, I would say one really interesting example has to do with perfect pitch.

So perfect pitch as a trait, that is to say, you have the ability to hear a note played and say,

oh, that's a C sharp. This is a pretty rare trait. So even if you look among highly trained

musicians, if you went to Peabody Conservatory at my university at Johns Hopkins and tested

people there, you would find a higher incidence of perfect pitch than you would in the general

population. But still, maybe one in 10 trained musicians have perfect pitch. And parenthetically,

having perfect pitch doesn't necessarily make you a better musician, but it's an interesting

phenomena. And so the question is, well, is perfect pitch heritable? And the answer is, when you

look at twin studies, which is what we use to estimate heritability, the answer is, it's kind

of low. There's a heritable component, but it accounts for my recollection is on the order of

30, 40 percent of the variability in perfect pitch. However, if people receive ear training

starting at a young age, the chance that they will develop perfect pitch can improve drastically.

In your book, Unique, do you cover aspects of human individuality that extend beyond the

perception domain into the cognitive domain? Well, yeah, absolutely. And I think it's good

to set the stage here if we're going to be talking about heritability and human individuality.

And so if I can go off on a little bit of a riff for the benefit of your listeners and viewers here.

So if you look at human traits, whether they are behavioral traits like shyness or very

straightforward morphological traits like height, what you tend to find is that there are very few

traits that are entirely heritable, where all their variability can be predicted based on the

gene variants you get from your mother and father. And there are a few traits that are

absolutely unheritable, but that most fall in between. So let me give an example. Everyone in

the world has either wet or dry earwax. And it turns out that this is determined by variation

in the single gene. The name of the gene is boring. It's ABCC11. It's an ion transporter.

And there's a variation in this gene gives rise to either wet or dry earwax. It doesn't matter

how your parents raise you. It doesn't matter what foods you ate growing up. It doesn't matter what

diseases your mother had when you were on the womb. It's 100% heritable. Well, does this mean

that ABCC11, we should call it the earwax type gene? Well, no, because it's not there just for

that. Like this gene is expressed in cells in all parts of the body doing all kinds of things.

Earwax is just something that we notice. Genes don't code for traits. They code for proteins.

And so we have to be careful about how we refer to them in that way. For example, the wet earwax

variant of the ABCC11 gene also confers a slightly higher risk for breast cancer.

So clearly, it's not just for earwax. It's for a bunch of things, most of which we don't yet

know about. But in the case of earwax, this trait is 100% heritable. At the other end of the scale,

speech accent is 0% heritable. It is entirely dependent upon the speech that you experience.

In your childhood. And interestingly, it's the speech of your peers more than the speech

of your family, which is why the children of immigrants sound like the place where they wound

up, not like their parents. And there is no evidence for any degree of heritability. Now,

just to be clear, I'm talking about speech accent, like whether you have a high or a low voice or

it's nasal or more or less resonant, these are physical things having to do with the vocal tract

and they are in part heritable. Okay, so we've got one thing that's 100% heritable and one thing

that's 0% heritable. But where do most things fall? Most things fall in the middle. One of the

most heritable traits that we know about in humans is height. And in the United States,

height is about 85% heritable. 85% of the variation in the trait of height can be

explained by what you inherit from your mother and your father. Well, what's the rest? Well,

it's nutrition. It's the diseases you fought off. It's also random variation, which we'll talk about

a lot later. Now, you might say, okay, well, that's an estimate for people in the US. Is

this true all over the world? Well, no. If you go to a place where people routinely

don't get enough nutrition and are routinely fighting off infectious diseases like this has

been studied in rural Bolivia, for example, or rural India. Now, height is no longer 85% heritable.

It's only 50% heritable. Why? Because people in these situations where they don't get enough

nourishment or they're fighting off these diseases can't live up to their genetic potential

for height. If you want to make things better for the people of the world, then everyone needs to have

basic things like the ability to learn enough nutrition and decent medical care and schools

in order to fulfill their genetic potential for positive traits. And height, I've used as an

example because it is very uncontroversial, but we could apply the very same analysis to intelligence,

general intelligence. Now, there are people who argue about do things like IQ tests really

measure anything real, and there's been a lot of fighting in the scientific literature about this.

But I think intelligence tests aren't perfect, and they are sometimes culture bound, but they

are actually quite predictive of later success. And much more so than, say, SAT tests or GRE tests or

MCATs or other standardized tests. But presumably those correlate in some way.

They do. But the IQ tests are better, actually. They're talking about the classic IQ test.

I am talking about the modern variants of the classic IQ tests that are administered by trained

psychologists and aren't just a paper form. And so they're not perfect, and no test will be perfect,

but they're pretty good. And so then if you ask the question, well, what is the heritability

for IQ test score? Well, the answer tends to be different, depending upon the population.

If you look, again, in countries like the U.S. or in Western Europe that are fairly affluent,

where people tend to have good access to nutrition and medical care and schooling,

and kids get to play, and they're not traumatized by war, then IQ test score is heritable in the

ballpark of 60, 70%. But if you look at people who don't have those benefits, who are poor,

and this can be in the United States as well, if you look at communities that face discrimination

and have consistently poor, poor healthcare in schools, then IQ is less heritable. Why,

for the very same reason that it is in height? Because people can't live up to their genetic

potential when they don't have the basic things that everybody needs. So presumably, if two identical

twins, and I realize they aren't identical, but you're familiar with twins, you have twin children,

if two identical twins are raised separately, the correlation in their IQ is that only 60,

I think you said about 66% of their IQ can be predicted on the basis of their genetic makeup

alone. I mean, it makes perfect sense to me as to why if one of those twins went to schools that were

demanding of a lot of different topic matter, and the other one went to schools where the

instruction level was really deficient, that one would perform far less well on an IQ test,

unless of course the IQ test isn't tapping into school-based knowledge, it's tapping into some

other thermometer of so-called intelligence or IQ. Well, the thing is that good schools

correlate with many other things, right? So the students that go to good schools aren't just

benefiting from good schools, they tend to also have good medical care and safer, less traumatizing

neighborhoods, and they're more likely to have parents with books in the home and a whole number

of things that are all beneficial. So when you try to do epidemiology on this, you have to be aware

that things are very deeply interconnected, but you bring up a good point. So it turns out that

the way we get these estimates of heritability, there's two ways. One way is to compare

so-called identical or monozygotic twins with so-called fraternal or dizygotic twins. So

the identical twins will share nearly 100% of their gene variants, and on average,

fraternal twins share 50% of their gene variants. And generally speaking, when people do these studies

in order to avoid confounds of sex, they'll compare same sex fraternal twins, so boys to boys

and girls to girls. And when you put these incidents into a formula called Fisher's Equation,

then you can come up with an estimate of the heritability of the trait. But there is an assumption

present in that, and it's called the Equal Environment Assumption. You're saying, well,

two kids raised in the same family have the same environments. Well, that's not always true,

right? That can be violated by a number of different situations. So it turns out that a

more powerful but much more difficult way to estimate heritability is by looking at twins

reared apart, either identical twins or fraternal twins reared apart. And there was a landmark

study called the Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart, which is abbreviated MISTRA. That is really

the gold standard for assessing the heritability of many different human traits, both behavioral

traits, but also disease incidents. But of course, it's a small M because the population

of identical twins reared apart that you can get into the lab isn't that large. They had something,

I don't remember the exact numbers, but they had something like 80-some

identicals and 50-some fraternals in their sample. But by doing this, they were able to

come up with a lot of interesting estimates. And so, for example, most personality traits,

what the psychologists use the acronym ocean to mean openness, conscientiousness, empathy,

agreeableness, and neuroticism. I think I got that right. Spelled right.

That these traits, on average, tend to be about 50% heritable. And so, okay, say, right, well,

50% of those personality traits is heritable. The rest has got to be like how you were raised.

It's got to be in your family. And so, everyone was shocked when they actually did the analysis

and found that family has almost nothing to do with it.

And what are you kidding? It's got to. And I think the important thing to realize is these

traits I just listed, we call these personality traits, but they are not the sum total of the way

you are in the world. Parents can inculcate many things in their children. They can demonstrate

trades. So, people are much more likely to go into an occupation if their parents did.

They can inculcate moral ideas and religious ideas. But in terms of these ocean personality traits,

they have astonishingly little to do with it. So then, this brings up the question, well,

if 50% of the variation in these personality traits is not from your genetics and it's not

from your family, where does it come from? And the answer seems to be is that it comes from

the random nature of the development of the body and the nervous system. And this is a point that

I think many people don't understand. This is something that biologists know, but we've done a

very poor job of communicating to the general public. The genome, all your DNA, all 3 billion

bases of DNA, all 19,000 or so genes in a human, don't make a blueprint for making your body and

brain. It's not a schematic diagram that connects everything to everything, particularly in the

nervous system where we have these hundreds of trillions of connections. Rather, it's a rather

vague recipe. So, the genome doesn't say, oh, okay, you glutamate using neuron in the brain region

called the thalamus, you know, grow for 200 microns towards the top and then cross the

midline and then grow towards the ear for another distance. No, it says something like,

hey, you bunch of glutamate neurons in the thalamus over here in this area,

about half of you cross the midline. And so what does this mean in terms of individual variation?

It means, well, for some individuals, 40% of their axons will cross the midline of the brain.

And for another individual, 60% will, even in identical twins. And as you correctly said a

moment ago, identical twins aren't really identical either in their bodies or their temperament.

So, if you take newborn identical twins and you give them a CT scan just to measure the shape

of their organs, they're not the same. You might have one twin whose spleen is 30% larger than the

other twins or whose liver is 30% smaller than the other twins, even though they have the exact

same DNA and they were lying right next to each other in the womb and presumably had the same or

very similar fetal environment. And the reason is the random or as we say stochastic nature of

neural development. A great way to study this is with nine banded armadillos. I know we're

getting weird here. I love the armadillo because I've been told, tell me, I don't want to interrupt

you too long, but as far as I know, the only animal in North America that carries leprosy?

That is true. And there's a lot of twinning going on in armadillos, right?

Well, what there is is actually quadding. So, armadillos or the nine banded armadillo in particular,

and they're different armadillos. I'm not really an armadillo specialist, so I don't know if this

holds for all of them, but the nine banded armadillo is born as identical quadruplets.

Awesome. Awesome. So, you can take these identical quadruplets, newly born armadillo. I don't know

if you think you could call them pups. I don't know what a baby armadillo is called. I'm sure

there's some particular word for it. And... Someone will tell us in the comments on YouTube,

what is the name of a baby armadillo? I know like a ferret, baby ferrets are kits.

The moms are jills, the dads are bobs. I used to be obsessed with this kind of naming, you know,

it's a business of ferrets or what is like a gang of raccoons or whatever. So, if you can tell us

what the name is for the baby armadillos, as well as what do you call a group of armadillos,

you win the pride associated with being right. That's right, right.

One of my favorites is an ostentation of peacocks. Amazing.

Or a murder of crows. Who comes up with this stuff? I know, it's a good...

Or a raft of otters. I think that's correct. So, if you have four newborn identical nine banded

armadillos, then this is a great model system that biologists can use to study

stochastic differences in development. And sure enough, their brains are wired

slightly differently, their bodies are slightly different. If you test them behaviorally,

even very, very early in life, they have different propensities. Some are bolder,

will explore more, some will tend to hide in the corner more. And, you know, we know this from the

lab. You get a box of mice that are inbred from the breeder and you pluck them out and they're

not behaviorally identical. Some might try to bite your hand, some will run away,

some will stand stock still. Where does this behavioral variation come from in mice that are

nearly genetically identical? Well, it comes from a bunch of things. They don't always have

exactly equal experience, but mostly it comes from the pseudo random stochastic nature of development.

I'd like to take a quick break and acknowledge our sponsor, Inside Tracker.

Inside Tracker is a personalized nutrition platform that analyzes data from your blood and DNA

to help you better understand your body and help you meet your health goals. I'm a big believer

in getting regular blood work done for the simple reason that many of the factors that impact

your immediate and long-term health can only be analyzed from a quality blood test. However,

with a lot of blood tests out there, you get information back about blood lipids, about hormones,

and so on, but you don't know what to do with that information. With Inside Tracker,

they have a personalized platform that makes it very easy to understand your data, that is,

to understand what those lipids, what those hormone levels, et cetera mean, and behavioral

supplement, nutrition, and other protocols to adjust those numbers to bring them into the

ranges that are ideal for your immediate and long-term health. Inside Tracker's ultimate plan now

includes measures of both ApoB and of insulin, which are key indicators of cardiovascular health

and energy regulation. If you'd like to try Inside Tracker, you can visit insidetracker.com

slash huberman to get 20% off any of Inside Tracker's plans. Again, that's insidetracker.com

slash huberman to get 20% off. And is the pseudo-random stochastic nature of development

one of the major driving forces for evolution? Because we hear about mutations and we always

think, or people tend to think rather, that mutations are bad. But of course, mutations

provide the variation that can also serve adaptive traits. I mean, if you're a fan of the X-Men,

as I am, huge fan of the X-Men, the entire series, every single one, including the Wolverine movies,

you quickly come to learn that genetic mutation is at the heart of variation, which is at the

heart of individuality, which is what we're talking about. Right. And so genetic variation

is at the heart of individuality, but there is also, there are also these other things,

right, that we've talked about. There's the effects of early life experience,

and there is the stochastic nature of development, because if you, through the randomness of

development, happen to have like a particularly great liver, you're not going to pass that on

to your children, right? That isn't in your germline. You won't pass that trait along.

Just a brief insert here on germline. We had Odette Racheffi on the podcast who studies

epigenetic transmission and inheritance of, it's not Lamarckian, we have to point that out,

but inheritance of sort of acquired traits does happen. And the germline, it's the genes that

are present in the sperm and in the eggs. All the other cells of your body have genes, of course,

but the best way to put this is simply going to the gym and getting fit does not make your

children more fit, because the germline, as far as we know, is not modified in a direct way.

In other words, the DNA within sperm and eggs are not modified according to your behaviors

in most, but not all cases. That's right. And as you correctly said about Odette's work and other

people's work, there is what's called transgenerational epigenetic inheritance, which means that you

can have traits that are passed not from one generation to the next, but even two generations

to the grandchildren that don't require modification of DNA. But to date, that has been shown very

convincingly in worms and in plants. The evidence in mammals is really not there yet, in my opinion.

And most of the claims for that, and it's a very popular thing to say, I epigenetically inherited

my grandmother's or great-grandmother's trauma, the evidence at present is poor. Actually, a lot of

it comes from epidemiology, most of which came from famines in the Överkalix region of northern

Sweden. And they had very good medical records, and they said, oh, well, if your grandfather

went through the famine, then you're more likely to have this trait if you're male, or if your

grandmother went through this, then if you're male, you have this trait and you're female,

you have that trait. And I mean, there are two problems. One is that there's not a biological

mechanism. But the other problem is that the way these things were discovered is by something

called harking or hypothesizing after the results are known. They did very many statistical comparisons

to try to find something significant. And you know from your work in the lab that when you do many

comparisons, you're going to get some things that look significant just occasionally randomly

through luck. And you have to apply a statistical correction called a Bonferroni correction when

you make many particularly post-hark comparisons after the experiment comparisons to set the bar

much higher for accepting that data. And most of those studies, they didn't apply that correction.

And I remain unconvinced of transgenerational epigenetic inheritance in mammals.

Now, let's be clear. Just because it hasn't been shown convincingly now doesn't mean that

it won't be. There are some good people working very hard on this, and they may well describe

a mechanism and show this convincingly in the years to come. But right now,

you may well inherit your grandma's or great grandma's trauma, but you're probably doing it

socially, not through marks on your DNA that changes how your genes are expressed or not

expressed. Yeah, so I subscribe to the idea that there's absolutely certainly transgenerational

inheritance of parenting and upbringing, right? I mean, your grandparents raise your parents who

raise you. Not always people can be adopted. In fact, I've adopted members of my family.

But if I understand what you're saying correctly, the evidence that, for instance,

some stress-related gene was modified during a trauma in my grandparents or great grandparents,

and the idea that that was passed to me through my parents, that the evidence there is far weaker.

Right, and when you think about it, well, how did that happen? That had to get into

your grandparents' sperm or egg cell and then produce that effect in the brain of your parents,

and then it had to get into their sperm or egg cell and then contribute to producing it in you.

But fragmentation of DNA in sperm or in eggs is a common thing, especially as people age,

DNA, sperm and eggs fragment, and it's possible that some of those mutations still allow for

viable embryos. So in theory, the germ line could be changed by environmental events.

Well, right, but now I think you're starting to talk about things that are heritable,

right? You're not talking about Marx on DNA, you're talking about the structure of the DNA

itself, and that is its own separate issue. Now, I want to be really careful about this,

because what is now, I think, fairly well-established is that you can transfer things

epigenetically over a single generation if, as a result of experiences that the mother has

during pregnancy. So, for example, we know that during the 1918 pandemic flu,

many women were pregnant and got the flu, and if you look at their children,

you find interesting statistical anomalies. And those children, for example, the males

wound up going into the army for World War II, and of course, the army does a complete physical,

and the records are very good, so you can go into that database. And you find that the male children

that were in utero during the winter of 1918, during the pandemic flu, are on average a millimeter

or two shorter. You might say a millimeter or two, that's nothing, but in a huge statistical

sample of millions of people, that's enormously significant. More interesting is that the incidence

of schizophrenia went up about four fold from about 1% to about 4%. And even though autism

wasn't a term in 1918 yet, I don't think it came along later, what we now retrospectively would

call autism also went up by about four fold. So, there's something about mom being stressed

and carrying the fetus at a particular stage that seems to impact brain development in a way

that then makes that child more likely to be schizophrenic or autistic when they grow up.

Do we know that it's stress? And my recollection of this, I believe this was the late Paul Sternberg's

work as well, I maybe have that name incorrect, but in any event, that it is if pregnant mom gets

the flu in the first trimester, you see this higher incidence of schizophrenia and autistic

offspring. But do we know that it's stress per se? Because it's stressful to have the flu,

but the flu is a bunch of other things. It could be fever, could be some breakdown in the immune

barrier. I just want to open up the number of variables that this could be, or do we know that

it's something in the hypothalamic pituitary so-called stress axis that is adrenals, so

hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis? Is it elevated cortisol, or could it literally be

an immune neural interaction of some other sort? It's probably the last thing you mentioned,

an immune neural interaction. And the reason I say that is that Gloria Choi at MIT,

together with her collaborators, has made a mouse model of this phenomena. So she takes

pregnant female mice, and she injects them with something that she doesn't actually infect them

with virus. She puts a chemical in that is on the code of viruses that mimics viral infection.

And then what happens is that in a way that interestingly is an interaction with the

bacterial content of her gut produces a surge of an immune signaling molecule called interleukin 17.

Interleukin 17 can pass through the placenta into the fetus, and if it's present, just as you said,

at a particular point in development, it doesn't work anywhere during pregnancy,

but during something that is sort of the mouse equivalent of the first trimester,

if that occurs, it causes disorder development of the layers of the cortex. Instead of it looking

like layers of a cake, you see balls and clumps of cells. And parenthetically, in some but not all

postmortem tissue from autistic people, you can also see those balls and clumps of cells.

Are those balls and clumps of cells thought to reflect alterations in cell migration?

They are, yes. I don't know if it's entirely known how much of it is cell division or migration,

but certainly migration is a part of it. And it's very likely that that critical moment

to disrupt this ordering of the brain and produce these increases in schizophrenia or

or autism vulnerability are coming at a point where neurons are migrating

during development. And so what Choice Group did is they did all the things you would want to do

as a biologist. So they gave things to block the function of this interleukin signaling molecule,

and it blocked the phenomenon. They artificially injected the signaling molecule into fetal brain

when the mom hadn't been stressed and they could they could reproduce it. So I'm not saying that

there aren't effects of stress hormones from the hypothalamic pituitary axis that are important

that you mentioned. But in this mouse model system work, it seems that you can produce it through

this immune signaling pathway. And so then the question is, well, like are these mice autistic?

Well, how do you know if a mouse is autistic? And the answer is, it's actually a little vague,

right? There are behaviors that neuroscientists say are analogous of human autism. And one of them is

if you give a mouse a marble in its home cage, it will bury it over and over again, compulsively,

or bury many marbles. And people say that that is somehow analogous to some of the

compulsive behaviors you see in autism. It's a bit of a stretch, right? I mean, it's a challenge

to interpret mouse behavior in in human terms, but it's a reasonable first step.

Incredible. And I hope more will continue to be done as it surrounds the

first trimester influenza hypothesis, because it's been around a while. And obviously,

there's a spectrum of what we call autism mass burgers. And nowadays, people refer to it as

sometimes as neuro atypical. There's some high functioning people with autism. There's some

low functioning people with autism. And for that matter, there's some high functioning and low

functioning people who don't have autism. So but it is something that I think demands our attention.

And that hopefully will be resolved at some point, because also influenza is but one immune

insult. And presumably pregnant women are being bombarded with all sorts of viruses and bacteria

and fungal infections and fighting them off or not fighting them off. And who knows what the

variation in neuro immune interactions exist in there that give rise to good variation

and let's call it debilitating variation. Well, that's absolutely right. And so for

one example is that we don't know what the effects are on the children who were in utero

while their mothers were fighting off COVID. We won't know for a while. Or the calm and cold.

And there might, I mean, there might be nothing, but or there might be something serious

lurking there like there was for pandemic flu. And will be very interesting and important to

find out. Agreed. I'd love to talk with you about mind body. But before we do that, I would be

totally remiss if I didn't ask for your broad top contour understanding of the mini brain,

the cerebellum, right, the so called mini brain. And here's why I've been a practicing neuroscientist

for, you know, close to three decades. I know where the cerebellum is. I've dissected a bunch of them.

I can tell you where a few things are in there. And I certainly have read about what the cerebellum

does. But whenever I do a PubMed search on cerebellum, I see an ever expanding set of

things that the cerebellum is implicated in, not just balance, as most people hear, but also

timing, also cognition. I hear about timing in particular of motor behavior. But then I also

hear that it's involved in learning and not just motor learning. And it certainly is involved in

motor learning. Perhaps that little mini brain is doing 50 or a thousand different things.

But how should we think about the cerebellum? What is it doing? And what are some of its

core operations that inform both what it's doing and perhaps what other areas of the brain are

doing as well? Because I can point to the retinas or to auditory cortex or the thalamus. And yes,

there's some mysterious nuclei in the brain. But to me, the cerebellum is one of the most cryptic

and complicated structures to understand. And I know you spent some time in there. So

what's the cerebellum do? Well, cerebellar researchers like to joke that the cerebellum

is a counterweight to keep your head from falling forward. Oh, perfect. Well, with all the texting

nowadays, people need bigger cerebellums. That's right. That's right. Probably over many generations,

then that will happen along with an expansion of the thumbs. But of course, everybody is going to

text with their mind in about another 10 years, right? You'll have Elon's implant and you won't

need your thumbs at all. Or maybe just five. Or my friend Eddie Chang is a neurosurgeon and

works on the auditory system. He's been on this podcast before. He was saying that in theory

and actually in practice, you could just record the neural output to the muscles of the speech

system, essentially. And you could just amplify that and you could text without actually speaking.

In fact, when we read, he told me, we are actually receiving the signals

as if we were going to speak the words we're reading, but they don't quite arrive at the

place where you could get full blown post synaptic potential. So you're not actually

moving the vocal machinery. So that means the motor signals are getting sent out there

and so you're speaking what you are reading, but you just don't know it.

That's right. And it's very analogous to what happens during the REM phase of sleep.

When you have commands, your brain is issuing commands to your muscles to do things like behave

in your dreams, to run away or go here or go there. But those signals actually are blocked in the

brainstem prevented from reaching your muscles because the nerves that don't go through your

brainstem, like the ones that control your eye movements, aren't subject to that blockade.

That's why you can produce the rapid eye movements in REM sleep. But yeah, this is a

general theme in the brain. A lot of times you have the output, but then you shut it down.

And there are REM sleep behavior disorders where people thrash and move in their sleep

during REM. And it's because this outflow that's normally blocked isn't blocked.

Have you ever had the reverse happen? I have where you wake up and you're still in so-called

REM atonia, you're still paralyzed. And there's that split second that feels like eternity where

you are wide awake and you cannot move. And I'll tell you, it's terrifying.

Yeah, sleep paralysis. And actually, it's been known forever. You can actually find ancient Greek

depictions of people lying with a demon on their chest paralyzing them. And that is actually from

sleep paralysis. Later, Hogarth did a drawing of exactly that. So yeah, this is a well-known

phenomenon. But to get back to the cerebellum as we started. So

the cerebellum is, as you said, definitely involved in motor coordination. So people who

have damage to the cerebellum aren't paralyzed, but they tend to be clumsy. They tend to not

coordinate their movements. They have a disturbed gait. If they're reaching for an object, they

often overshoot it and have to make successive approximating motions to get back to their target.

So that's well understood. But if you look through evolution, the cerebellum is connected

to this brain region called the thalamus. And it's connected from there to many regions, including

the frontal cortex where phenomena like planning and decision making and moral sense and many

aspects of personality seem to be encoded. So then the question becomes, well, that's very far away

from being clumsy. What are these connections doing? And as time has gone on, I've been in

this business for over 40 years now. I'm an old guy. And so initially we said, oh yeah, cerebellum

movement control, motor coordination, that's what it's for. As time goes on, as you correctly said,

the cerebellum has been implicated in more and more functions, many of which are far removed

from the motor system. And if we're looking for a theme about what the cerebellum does is that

it is there to predict the immediate future. It's trying to determine what's going to happen

in the next second or two to best guide behavior. And as you can imagine, this kind of

general computation could be applied very well. You can see why it's important for motor systems

and doing sports. And if you're trying to hit a baseball and looking at what the pitcher is doing

and trying to anticipate what the pitch is. But it also comes up in a social realm. If we're trying

to read someone and predict what they are going to do is this person, friend or foe,

which is one of the first things that we try to assess when meeting someone, are they competent,

which is the second thing we try to assess when meeting something. A lot of this depends upon

predictive circuitry. And it depends in part on the cerebellum. And it seems to be at least partially

impaired in people who sustain cerebellar damage. So it seems as if, interestingly,

the cerebellum started out for prediction related for motor control and through evolution,

its basic computation has been applied to other non-motor behaviors. Now, I'm speaking in generalities

and a lot of the details of this remain to be worked out and understood. But I would say that is

in a nutshell the modern conception of the cerebellum.

Thank you. Finally, somebody explains to me in a top contour but highly informed way

what the cerebellum does. I couldn't be more grateful.

My pleasure. In all aspects of biology and life, the term nature versus nurture is relevant, but

never so much as when thinking about the nervous system. And I know this firsthand because I've

studied neural development, both the nature side, the so-called hardwired stuff that genes just set

up, neurons wire up to that neuron, et cetera, the cerebellum's in the back, the eyes are in the front,

the hardwired stuff. And then the softwired stuff, the nurture stuff is the stuff that can be modified

by experience. What are your thoughts on nature versus nurture and should there even be a versus

in there? Yeah, I don't think there should. And I have a lot of problems with nature versus

nurture as an expression. It was popularized by Francis Galton in the 19th century, a colleague of

Darwin's. And I think it's wrong in or misleading in a lot of ways. So, of course, the nature in

nature versus nurture is meant in this case to mean heritability, right? What you inherit

in the gene variations from your mother and father. And nurture means like how your parents

or your community raised you. The problem I have with nurture is that it is too narrow a term,

that really it should be replaced with the word experience and experience in the broadest possible

sense, not just social experience, but the foods your mother ate when she was carrying you in utero.

The diseases you fought off or your mother fought off while she was carrying you in utero,

the bacterial population of your gut. So, experience meaning anything that impinges on you

starting from the earliest stages of fetal development, continuing to the last day of your

life. I think it should be very expansive, much more than social experience in the family or the

community. And as you mentioned, I have a problem with the versus because there's this idea that

these things are essentially in opposition. Well, is he that way because of his gene variants or is

that way because of what happened to him? And I think the thing to realize is that experience

and heredity interact in all kinds of interesting ways, some of which are oppositional and some of

which are reinforcing. A classic one from genetics has to do with a genetic disease

called phenylketonuria or PKU, which is an inability to metabolize the dietary amino acid

phenylalanine. And so, in order to have this, you have to inherit broken copies of this gene

from both your mother and your father. So, it's a so-called recessive trait. And here's where the

experience comes in. It only matters if you eat foods rich in phenylalanine. If you don't,

it doesn't matter that you inherited these things. So, that's a way in which genes and

experience interact. An idea of ways in which then they interact positively, think about

athletic ability, right? So, a lot of athletic ability has a heritable component. If you are born

fast, for example, then you're more likely to do sports and practice them and get better

at sports as a result of your experience. So, here genes and experience are feeding back on

each other in a positive feedback loop. So, there really isn't a versus at all. And then,

of course, the last thing is that this isn't the entirety. So, we talked earlier about the

pseudorandom nature of development, stochastic development. And so, if I were to take the

phrase nature versus nurture and reconfigure it, I would change it to read

heritability interacting with experience filtered through the random nature of development. Now,

that doesn't fall off the tongue as elegantly as nature versus nurture. You know, nature versus

nurture is like, if the gloves don't fit, you must quit. You know, it's got that kind of snappy

snare drum beat. But I think it's a more accurate.

So, heritability interacting with experience filtered through the randomness of development.

So, we can shorten that up and we'll just call it the Linden hypothesis.

You know, I don't think I can take credit for that. It belongs to other people.

Sure. But there's a long history and science of things being shortened up and

coined. And that's as important. We're not trying to rob attribution here.

And the good news is, perhaps you can't call it the Linden hypothesis, but I can.

And what I found is as with the Galpin equation, which is now out there as a hydration,

it's a formula that gives broad but research informed parameters as to how much water one

should drink in order to maintain proper hydration for physiologist Dr. Andy Galpin,

who's a PhD in physiology and an expert in all aspects of exercise science.

There's the Galpin equation. There's the sobered principle. So, I'm naming these things left

and right where appropriate. And so, I'm naming these things sparingly and where

appropriate. So, from here on out, heritability, interacting with experience, filtered through

the randomness of development is the Linden hypothesis. And I'll be damned if anyone's

going to rename it faster than I'm going to propagate it.

All right. Well, I think all the geneticists will be gnashing their teeth about this being

named after someone who isn't actually a geneticist.

Quite all right. And their dentists will thank me. Let's talk about mind body.

Okay. I'm fascinated by this for a couple of reasons and I promise to keep this brief. But

when I was growing up, I was very interested in animals and biology and my father's a scientist

and I got very interested in neuroscience early as people perhaps know. And so much of neuroscience

as I was coming up through the mid 90s, 2000s, 2010 to 20 stretch was focused on the brain piece.

Very little on the body. There was nothing about gut brain access in the early discussions and

coursework. In parallel to all of that, I've been interested in mental health, physical health,

and let's just call it performance and got interested in meditation, respiration-based

practices, things like yoga, nedra, things that by way of experience, I understood immediately

had a profound influence on the nervous system, states of mind and body.

Nowadays, there's an entire institute at the National Institutes of Health

for complementary health and medicine, essentially exploring things like yoga,

nedra, respiration practices, even supplements and things of that sort. And there's this

understanding that, oh my goodness, the nervous system extends into the body and the body sends

neural signals back into the brain. And so this whole notion of mind body has fortunately migrated

away from California counterculture, Esalen Institute only hippie new age magic carpet stuff.

By the way, that's not what I believe, but that's often how it was looked at in the past.

And now people at every level of science and medicine at every major university

and in every scientific journal are starting to publish papers about the interactions between

bodily organs, like the breathing apparatus, the diaphragm lungs, the heart, heart rate

variability we hear about, the liver, the gut brain axis in particular. And so mind body,

the idea that our thoughts could influence our body and that our bodily state could influence our

thoughts is fortunately not just understood, but it seems to be both accepted and appreciated.

So what are your thoughts on mind body? What does that mean to you? And what do you think is

the potential of the mind body interaction? It seems to me we've just barely scratched the surface.

Yeah, well, I'm glad you asked, because I think it's a really fascinating

situation and where things are changing very, very quickly. And I think to me,

the most important thing for people to understand is that when you have a hypothesis,

let's say you have a hypothesis that meditation can attenuate chronic pain.

Well, there is a temptation to think that this operates outside the realm of science and biology,

that is, in some airy, fairy realm in the clouds that this happens. And I mean, for good reason,

there are a lot of people who will describe it in exactly that way with auras or they

co-op scientific terms like resonance and energy, but they don't actually use them in scientific

ways. So there's a lot of very fuzzy language that surrounds this, but it shouldn't obscure the fact

that when you have a hypothesis that, say, some mental state like meditation or guided breathing

affects some process in the body, that you should be trying to understand this in terms of a biological

hypothesis, not in terms of some indistinct realm that is different.

And I really learned this initially from my father. My father was a psychiatrist,

in fact, kind of a talking cure old-fashioned psychoanalyst who had his practice in Los Angeles.

And we would have dinner together every Wednesday night. And he would always tell me about his

patience. He was very careful to keep confidentiality. He wouldn't break confidentiality, but I would

say, oh, yes, how's your narcissist? Oh, we had this dream. So this was normal conversation when

I was 14, 15 years old with my dad. And one day I said, dad, it's really clear to me that through

this talking cure, a large fraction of your patients feel better and they conquer their

depression or their obsessive thoughts or things that are blocking them. How do you think it works?

And he says, well, we don't really know the mechanics, but ultimately when it works,

it's not working in some airy-fairier realm. It is working by changing the biology of the brain.

And when he said that, it was like a lightning bolt went off in my head. And I thought, well,

I don't have the kind of personality to be a talking cure psychiatrist. I'm not nearly nice

enough, but I could understand the underlying biology. Maybe I'll do that. And so as you've

correctly pointed out, when you say the phrase mind body, you're talking about two directions.

You're talking about mental functions affecting the body. And then you are also talking about how

phenomena in the body affect the mind. And we're understanding so much more about how that happens.

And I think the general thing for your listeners to appreciate is that we have some culprits

here, right? And generally speaking, there are two classes of culprits. So if you want to get

signals about the body to the mind, there's two ways to do that. One of them is through

neurons that reach out into the body and sense things. And this is referred to as introception,

right? So as opposed to extra reception, your outward pointing senses, these are the senses

that monitor your own body. And while we can consciously be aware of a lot of that information,

a lot of it is happening subconsciously. Like your breathing is happening automatically,

most of the time without you thinking about it. And that depends upon sensors about your blood

chemistry and the state of your lungs and a number of other things that are regulating that process.

And it's all happening in the brain, usually below the level of your conscious attention.

In addition to the neural signals, there is also a whole realm of hormonal or diffusible

immune signals. And what these are is that these are chemicals that are released into the blood

stream and that move throughout the body and that can activate neurons in the brain or in other

parts of the nervous system to produce changes in mental function. And I think the real thing

that is exciting a lot of people right now has to do with immune signaling molecules.

So there's a class of molecules called cytokines. And cytokines are basically the signaling

hormones of the immune system and they can flow through the bloodstream and through lymphatic

fluid and reach many parts of the body. We've known for a number of years that the specialized

receptors for these cytokines are found throughout the brain. And yet we know very, very, very little

about what they do. And that's going to be an astonishingly fruitful area of scientific research.

But to give one exemplar, there are a lot of things these days suggesting a link between

inflammation in the body, whether it be in the gut or in other places, to depression.

Well, how might that work? Well, it could work either through inflammation-sensing

neurons sending electrical signals to the brain or, and it's not either or, it could be both.

It could be immune signaling cytokine molecules produced at the site of inflammation that then

travel through the bloodstream and the lymphatic system to then reach the brain, bind receptors,

and have effects. And so one of the mysteries about depression is that it's not that tractable to

pharmacological therapy. So if you look at people who suffer with depression, about a third of people

see significant benefit from modern SSRI and related antidepressant drugs, about a third see

very tiny benefit and about a third see no benefit at all. And part of the reason is because maybe

our term depression is too big a bucket. Depression is actually many different biological disorders

and only a subset of those are are helped by SSRIs and will need different therapies

for the other ones. That's certainly part of it. But part of it might actually have to do with

inflammation. So if you think that inflammation is a risk factor in depression, well, you could do

something very simple, right? You could, you could gobble an ibuprofen, right? There's a whole bunch

of anti-inflammatory drugs that are very well understood. And so, well, what if you just say,

all right, you know, let's have a study where we have a bunch of depressed people and we have them

all eat anti-inflammatory drugs for a few weeks and we see if this relieves their depression. And

the answer seems to be no, it doesn't. Well, and that's a little bit hard to understand because

there are definitely links between inflammation and depression. So for example, one of the early

treatments for hepatitis C that's since been superseded by more modern drugs was a pro-inflammatory

cytokine molecule. And when you gave it to people to treat their hepatitis C, almost everyone

became depressed on this drug. So, oh, well, this really seems like, like, like a link. Likewise,

there are certain neurological diseases like multiple sclerosis. It turns out the incidence

of depression as a comorbidity in multiple sclerosis is enormous. And you might think, well,

there's a trivial reason for that. If you're paralyzed from MS, you're bummed out about life.

And that's the reason. But if you look at people who have spinal cord injuries from accidents,

they actually have major depression at a rate from people who are uninjured. So that doesn't

seem to be it. It's not just that you're bummed out from being paralyzed, although, of course,

it's reasonable to be bummed out about being paralyzed. But that's not it. So what happens in MS?

Well, there's a bunch of cytokines, including one called interleukin-6, IL-6. That's elevated

massively if you take a spinal tap and you look at cerebral spinal fluid. And so that could be

causative for depression. So all these real reasons to think that inflammation is involved,

but yet the idea is still a little messy. So now what, if instead of looking at the general

population of depressed people, you look at the subset of people that don't respond to SSRI

antidepressants, are they helped by anti-inflammatories? And there's a bit of a hint that maybe they

are. It's not definitive yet. There are a couple of studies. It's right on the edge. But I think

this is a really good example of how we are going to see progress very soon in the body-to-mind part

of mind body medicine that is going to be of enormous benefit to people.

So interesting. Could I get your thoughts on one candidate hypothesis that I've been thinking

about? I've covered depression on a few episodes, and I've had Robin Cardard-Harris from UCSF and

Dr. Matthew Johnson from your very own John Hopkins University, both of whom work run

laboratories studying psychedelics for the treatment of depression. The clinical trials on

psilocybin. And to be clear, psilocybin is still illegal. It's been decriminalized a few places,

but we're not talking about recreational use. We're talking about several therapy sessions,

and then two, without psilocybin, then 2.5 gram approximately dosages of psilocybin given separately,

again with therapists present, and then follow-up therapy sessions seem to lead to relief of

depression in approximately somewhere between 65 and 80% of people. In some cases, total remission,

in some cases, some relief without remission. Okay, so we can kind of set that result on the

shelf. It's been repeated a number of different times. Compare that to the results of SSRIs,

which seem to help a third of people, a third minimally, and a third not at all. And of course,

there's a side effect profiles of the SSRIs and associated drugs, not just the SSRIs,

but Pryron and the other antidepressants that are taken in prescription drug form.

And then there's this inflammation piece. So could we hypothesize that relief from depression

has something to do with neuroplasticity, rewiring of neural circuits, and that psilocybin we know

can encourage neuroplasticity, and that perhaps SSRIs can encourage neuroplasticity in some people,

not all, and that inflammation is a barrier to neuroplasticity. To me, this is the only thing

that can reconcile the current status of the results. And then there's ketamine-based therapies,

and so we have to also kind of set that on the shelf. But let's set that aside on the shelf

for now to keep it simple. It seems to me that based on the time course over which SSRIs work,

the fact that they increase serotonin very quickly, but the relief from depression comes

much later, the fact that neuromodulators like serotonin are intimately involved in

neuroplasticity, they can in some cases gait neuroplasticity, that it all centers back to

changing neural circuits. And so what we're really trying to do, whether or not it's transcranial

magnetic stimulation, or now we can throw ketamine in there or psilocybin or SSRIs,

that treating depression is about rewiring the brain, it's not about chemical A or B per se,

although serotonin seems involved. To me, what I'd love to see are more studies about

the interaction between neuroplasticity and inflammation. And are we seeing that kind of

work out there? And because these results sort of sit as disparate, somewhat conflicting,

but it seems like inflammation is anti-neuroplasticity. And broadly speaking here, I realize there

are many interleukins, there are many, some of which are inflammatory, some of which are

anti-inflammatory. But is that a meaningful hypothesis? And do you think there's any hope

whatsoever to actually cure depression if we start to unify the results in these different camps?

Yeah, I think it's a completely reasonable hypothesis. And I would be broader. And I would

say, honestly, the relief of any neuropsychiatric condition ultimately is from neuroplasticity

in some form or another. And I think it's worthwhile to step back a bit and talk about

what neuroplasticity means. To date, there has been a focus on synapses, on the contacts between

neurons as the site of neuroplasticity. And that's warranted. Synapses are plastic,

they change as a result of experience, as a result of hormone changes, as a result of exercise,

as a result of lots of things. But synapses are not the be-all and end-all of neural function.

So for example, neurons work by sending electrical signals along their lengths and between neurons

and interconverting those with chemical signals. And the processes of generating those electrical

signals, the ion channels that are involved, that are embedded in membranes, that are involved in

that, are also plastic. They can also change as a result of experience. That's what we call

intrinsic plasticity as opposed to synaptic plasticity. In addition, there are literal

morphological changes. So when we talk about the wiring of the brain, sometimes we're talking about

literal wiring, like cell A wasn't connected to cell B and now it is, and that changes. And then

sometimes, well, actually cell A was connected to cell B, but cell B wasn't responsive enough,

and now there's a change in cell B, so now cell A can fire cell B. And that could have been a result

of a change in its synapse, making it more receptive to a transmitter released from cell A,

or it could be something intrinsic in cell A that makes it fire its electrical signal, its spike

more easily. I think that one of the key cell types that's going to be important for your

hypothesis linking inflammation to synaptic plasticity is going to be a cell called a microglial

cell. And microglial cells are non-neuronal cells in the brain that are motile, they can crawl around,

they have long processes, and they can gobble things up. They can literally sort of chew away

and digest bits of the extracellular scaffolding that surrounds neurons and synapses,

and thereby renders them plastic. They can destroy synapses, and there is a lot of indication that

certain disease states may involve over-exuberant microglia pruning synapses to a degree that they

shouldn't. And we know that microglia are chock-full of cytokine receptors, and so are responsive

to inflammatory signals. When we're talking about inflammation and we're talking about drugs,

it's worthwhile to mention that there are a lot of behavioral things that also can influence the

signaling. So we know, and I know you've discussed on your program, the incredibly

solubrious effects of physical exercise on mental function. So exercise is about as good and

antidepressant as SSRIs are, and the side effects are only good side effects as opposed to the bad

side effects of SSRIs. And again, this isn't working through some airy farrier realm. The reason

that exercise works to relieve depression and the reason that exercise works to maintain

your cognitive function as you age is because of biological pathways that we are now uncovering,

some of which will involve microglial cells and neurons and other types of cells in the brains,

some of which will involve not the neurons in the brain at all, but the brain's vasculature.

So we know that exercise is very solubrious for keeping blood flowing to the brain. And when

you're young, you have a super abundance of blood flowing to your brain. So it doesn't matter if it's

reduced, transiently, you're fine. But as you get older, your blood vessels become more occluded and

less elastic. And you're closer to the trouble spot. And if you exercise regularly, you can

dilate and make your blood vessels, including those in your brain, more elastic. And that is

almost certainly protective against both depression and cognitive decline as we age.

Well, I am a fan of exercise, but I'm fortunate that I enjoy running in some forms of resistance

training. So I always assumed that the good side effects were just the positive mood effects until

the recent literature that, as you mentioned, improved vasculature blood flow and reduced

inflammation. If not during the exercise when inflammation actually increases, decreases

inflammation. I'm delighted to hear you say the word microglia and my postdoc advisor,

the late Ben Barris, would be especially delighted. People can look up Ben. I'll provide a link to his

biography in the show note captions because he really championed, to the point of, I don't know

if champion's even a sufficient word. I mean, Ben was beating the drum saying, we have to pay

attention to glia. We have to pay attention to glia for the longest time. Glia were relegated

to these other journals, they even had their own journals. And in the last, what is it, 10 years,

there's been a kind of explosion of research exploring the role of microglia and other glial

cell types. And it's really fantastic to see that this actually the most abundant cell type in the

brain is the glial cell are getting the attention they deserve. It's absolutely true. And we scientists

like to think that we're very rational creatures and we're not subject to fads, but we totally are.

When I started out in this field in the early 80s, everything was about opioid peptides.

And then there was a period where gaseous neurotransmitters like nitric oxide were all the

rage. And right now, glia are in the spotlight for good reason. I'm not trying to say that it

isn't worthwhile, but there is this phenomenon of things being fattish and people jumping on

bandwag. And so it happens both in terms of the subject we study, but also in terms of the

techniques we use. And right now, the technique that is most fattish involves

single cell expression profiling, that is creating a list of what genes are turned on

and how strong they're turned on in single cells and seeing how that changes in different cell

types and with experience. And it's a very, it's a very valuable technique, but one could argue

that it is perhaps a bit overused. And that 15 years from now, people will go back and say,

gosh, those folks in 2023 were really overdoing it with the single cell profiling.

And if anyone's thinking about getting into the field of neuroscience or another area of

biological or other research, I can just tell you that if you're starting your PhD or your postdoc,

take a look at whatever fad is happening now and just know that in five years, it will be

something different. And it takes you about five years to finish your PhD or postdocs. So pick

something different than what's fattish now and you'll land right on the money.

There's always a lot to do. You don't have to do what everyone else is doing here.

And indeed, the deletion test becomes relevant here. The deletion test,

as it was described to me by my colleague, E.J. Chichulinski at Stanford, is if you look around

and you see one or more groups doing what you want to do very well, just pick something else.

Your life's going to be a lot more pleasant. Absolutely. I agree with E.J. on that entirely.

Let's get back to mind body. There are a bunch of different domains of mind body. As you

so aptly pointed out, it's bi-directional. Mind informs the body. Body informs the mind.

But we could probably break this down into a respiration. So breathing, conscious patterns

of breathing, emphasizing inhales or emphasizing exhales, cyclic hyperventilating, etc.

Could also be thought patterns. A little bit harder to break those down, but

many, not all, but many forms of meditation involve having a very still body. Not all.

There's walking meditation, etc. But still body, focused mind. Kind of a, not a state that we are

in a lot of times unless we direct that state. There are still other mind body patterns of

communication through very still body, deep relaxation. Things like yoga,

nedra, non-sleep, deep rest. There's hypnosis. There's touch-based body-mind communication.

If we're going to talk about mind body, also we should refer to as body-mind,

how do we dive in and think about this? Because this is involving clearly a thousand different

neural pathways, not just the vagus nerve. It's, you know, typically people just kind of hang

their hat on mind body. It must be vagus nerve. And of course it involves the vagus, but the

vagus is an extensive set of pathways. So how do you like to frame up mind body? And what's most

intriguing to you about mind body communication, both in terms of the biology and its practical

applications? Well, I think just as we talked about how there are two potential pathways

in conveying signals from the body to the mind, there are also two potential pathways,

at least two potential pathways to conveying signals from the brain to the body. And they are

the neural signals that are conveyed by neurons that actually get there. And they are

hormones and neurotransmitters and cytokines that are released from the brain. And I should

mention that hormones are actually produced by neurons, including, for example, some of the

hormones that you think about as being produced as sex hormones, like estrogen is produced by

neurons in your brain, for example. So let me give an example that I think is a bit out there,

but I think is really, really fascinating. And this comes from the cancer world. And so

melanoma is a bad cancer. It kills a lot of people. It can spread. It's highly metastatic.

And we know that melanomas often become innervated. That is to say, they become

contacted by neurons and wrapped and receive signals from them. And we also know that if a

melanoma becomes innervated, then the prognosis for that patient is worse. It's more likely to grow.

It's more likely to spread. Well, how does that happen? Well, recently, there have been some reports

that show that neurons that innervate the melanoma don't act directly onto the tumor cells. Rather,

what they do is they secrete a signaling molecule that has a receptor on immune cells that are

patrolling the edges of the melanoma tumor and nibbling away at it. And when that signaling

molecule is released from the neuron, it shuts down or reduces that immune patrolling function.

And then as a consequence, the tumor can grow and spread and bud off more readily.

So this signal that comes from neurons is sending the ambulances home, so to speak.

Yeah, exactly. And the signal is something called calcitonin gene-related peptide or CGRP.

I'm familiar with CGRP from the domain of touch and its involvement in, I think,

itch perception. Do I have pain perception and itch perception?

Right. Interesting.

Yeah. And so, all right. So if neurons can affect the progression of cancer through their activity,

and these neurons in the periphery are ultimately connected to the brain

through a couple of different hops, then is it reasonable to hypothesize that mental processes

could affect cancer progression? So let's say we have a hypothesis, and it's a wild hypothesis,

and I want to just emphasize that there is not evidence for this, but let's make the hypothesis

that says that through meditative practice, you can slow the progression of certain tumors that tend

to get innervated, right? Well, right now, this is just kind of a wild idea, but I think the

important thing, as I've said before, that this is a wild idea with the biological substrate.

It's not like you meditate and magic happens and force fields open and the angels sing,

and then your tumor shrinks. This is, we are saying that activity in certain areas of the

brain is increased by this meditative practice and that this sends signals to this neuron and

this neuron that actually go to the tumor and make something happen through this biochemical

pathway that we have defined, right? And to me, this is speculative, but it's also

extraordinarily exciting, right? It opens a kind of investigation of mind to body signaling

that has received very little attention up to now.

Incredible. And I say incredible because while you're giving an example of CGRP and nerve

innervation and metastatic tumors, I absolutely love the idea that a phenomenon involving some

practice that could be put under the umbrella of mind body or body mind becomes something entirely

different when we're trying to hang that on the hook of a biological process. It's like something

fundamentally changes there, right? You know, it's amazing to me, for instance, that early on

psychedelics and breathwork, conscious breathwork were lumped together, cost people their jobs at

major universities. I won't name the universities because you work at one, I work at another and

there's a third one called Harvard. I guess I just named them. But nowadays, there are laboratories

at every single one of those institutions studying deliberate respiration on health,

as well as psychedelics and meditation for that matter, with the goal of understanding

what cytokines, what neurotransmitters, et cetera, change through defined pathways,

including vagus, but phrenic nerves and frontal cortex and all the stuff that is considered,

you know, classic rigorous neuroscience. So I do think we've entered a new era. So it's not

costing people their jobs anymore. It's actually giving people their jobs and it's federally funded,

which itself is also fantastic in my opinion. So we are in a new era. What do you think

needs to be done to really nail down the idea that how we think influences our biology, even

though it's a total duh, because everyone knows that chronic stress, for instance,

this can be detrimental. Short-term stress can actually be beneficial. And stress is a mental

process that essentially deploys chemicals in the body that then create other issues in the body

that then create shifts in mental process. You know, it's so obvious when it's spelled out,

but it's just remarkable to me how this has just been lumped in the category of, like,

woo science. And I can't quite figure out what needs to be done in order to convince people

that their nervous system includes stuff outside the skull and spinal cord. And of course, of course,

it would work this way. Well, right. And I think it is the job of biomedical researchers right now

to reclaim a lot of this from the realm of nonsense. And the problem is there has been

a lot of nonsense. And, you know, there's sort of a visceral reaction when someone says, oh,

yeah, well, you can do breathwork and it'll realign your chakras. And that is, you know,

what will reduce your anxiety, your gut inflammation. And I'm tempted to just go, oh,

shut up. But what if the chakras are collections of nerve innervation of bodily sphincters and,

you know, it could make sense. Right, it could. But there's got to be some biology. In some cases,

these analogies are rooted in something real. And in some cases, they're just made up bullshit.

Right. And I think the challenge is to have really rigorous scientific tests of these things to take

it back. And to be willing to say, all right, there have been a lot of claims, for example, made

about how mental processes can influence the body. And only a subset of those are going to be true.

And of that subset, it is our job to understand how they work, both to rationalize them, but

also to optimize them and make them better. I mean, there's no question that mental processes

affect the body. I mean, we know, for example, that if we just keep you awake and don't let

you sleep long enough, you'll die. And what will you die from? You'll die from sepsis,

because the barrier between your gut contents and your perineum will break down. Right? Well, so

how does that happen? Right? We're just starting to understand. There's a really dramatic

example, but there are going to be many more subtle examples. So you've mentioned breathwork

a couple of times. And I think this is really interesting. My colleagues who are interested

in respiration tell me that you can record in many different places in the brain, many different

places in the neocortex and in other regions, and find a signature of the breathing rhythm,

sort of as a background to neural activity. You can find it in the cerebellum, you can find it

in the frontal cortex, you can find it in the habenula, which is implicated in many things,

including depression, you can find it lots of places. So the idea that conscious modulation

of your breathing could have manifold effects on neural function, I think is reasonable,

given that kind of observation. Here, here. Let's talk about you a bit more. You've been

so gracious in covering this wide array of topics and with such eloquence, and I must say I've been

delighting in all of it. You are in a unique position these days, because if I understand

correctly, you've been diagnosed with a fatal illness. I suppose we've all been diagnosed

with a fatal illness of sorts, because we're all going to die sooner or later. If you're

willing, could you tell us the story of how that diagnosis came to be, what your initial

reaction was, and where things stand now, and perhaps we can explore some of the,

well, let's just say pleasant surprises that have emerged since that initial diagnosis?

Well, sure, I'd be happy to. So in the summer of 2020, in the dark days of COVID, when things were

looking really bad, I developed profound shortness of breath. I couldn't get up a flight of stairs

without huffing and puffing, and I thought, oh, well, I've got COVID, but I took COVID vests,

and they were all negative. I thought, oh, well, I must have COVID. I've got the symptoms of COVID.

I have respiratory issues. I'm feeling weak. It's got to be COVID. After a while, when this didn't

go away, my wife said, look, you've got to go into the doctor. This is crazy. You've got to find out

what's going on. I did. They hooked me up to an electrocardiogram, and they said, oh,

you've got atrial fibrillation, meaning that your heart is doing two beats every time it should do

one. So I have a very high heart rate, and when the heart beats that fast, it can't work very

effectively. There is enough time to recharge before the next beat comes. Now, it turns out

that there is a very straightforward therapy for this. Atrial fibrillation comes from

electrical signaling in the heart swirling about in a circle and reactivating part of

the heart muscle faster than it should. And so if you thread through a catheter in your groin

up through blood vessels, you can put in a little needle and use that to

cauterize into a blade, a tiny little strip of cells in the heart that will produce a barrier

that will prevent that aberrant return of electrical activity and will cure atrial fibrillation. So I

had that process, that ablation surgery, and sure enough, it cured my atrial fibrillation.

I was feeling terrific. And they said, oh, as a follow-up, come back a few months later,

and we'll do an echocardiogram to see how your heart looks. And they did. And they went, oh,

my God, there's this huge mass pressing against your heart. It's like the size of a coke can.

Here's what we think it is. We think it's a hadal hernia. We think your stomach has

poked up through the diaphragm muscle and it's nestling next to your heart. So the way we

diagnose this is kind of humorous. They say, chug this can of Diet Dr. Pepper and then quickly

get up on the table and we'll do the echocardiogram. And in the echocardiogram, we can see a signature

of the popping CO2 bubbles in your soda. And if we see those in the mass, then we know it's your

stomach. So I did it. I chugged it. I got up there. Oh, nope, it's not your stomach. They said, oh,

okay. Well, what we think this is is a teratoma. And a teratoma is a developmental

anomaly that you carry usually from fetal life, where there's a group of different cells that

gets in a place where it shouldn't during development and then grows. You've probably

heard about people who sometimes get like a tooth that grows hair in their abdomen.

Sometimes women have these around their ovaries and they're not malignant. They don't spread.

It's a fairly easy thing. But I had this enormous coke can pressing on my heart. It may have, we

don't know, have been the source of my atrial fibrillation to start with. But in order to deal

with this, I had to have open heart surgery. So I had the surgery and it was a big hairy deal.

I was told it would last about five hours. It turns out it lasted two days.

They had me on the heart lung bypass machine longer than you're supposed to,

because you're very likely to throw a clot and get a stroke. Unfortunately, that didn't happen.

I had very skilled surgeons at Johns Hopkins. It was bleeding so much that they couldn't close

the chest. So they had to do all the surgery and then just leave me anesthetized with my chest open

until the bleeding stopped and they go, so the surgery was a bear. And then I'm waiting to get

the pathology report back on the tissue they removed and it came back and it was bad news.

Sorry, it's not a teratoma. It's not benign. It is a kind of cancer called synovial sarcoma.

And synovial sarcoma is a moderately rare cancer and it usually affects the synovium,

which is the lining of the joints or some other places. It's pretty rare to have it happen in

the heart. There are a few examples. If you look in the biomedical literature for common cancers

like testicular cancer or breast cancer, there are huge tables of statistics for millions of

patients on what's been tried and what works and what the prognosis is. For synovial sarcoma of

the heart, there are only individual case reports. Oh, there's a guy in Kenya and he got him. That's

what happened. There's a woman in Minnesota and this is what happened with her. There are no

statistics because it's that rare. And the oncologist said, well, I think you've got

six to 18 months to live. Now, this was about now 27 months ago. So I've fortunately

exceeded that lifespan estimate. And I think we got to be clear also that, you know, being an

oncologist has got to be a terrible job for many reasons. But one of them is that you got to give

a lifespan estimate even if you really don't have the data to do it. You can't just say,

I won't do it, right? You got to do it. People expect it. So, you know, I'm not saying like,

oh, the oncologist was incompetent because I've outlived my estimate. You know, he was trying to

do something based on very little information, made his best guess. So, you know, I got this

information and I was furious. I was so angry. Heart cancer. Who the hell gets heart cancer? Is

that even a thing? Have you ever heard of somebody with heart cancer? Not until now.

No. Heart cancer. What the F? I've got heart cancer. This is, this is crazy. The time I was

59 years old, I've got a lot to do. I can't have heart cancer. And what was

I think transformative for me is that at the same time that I was feeling white, hot, angry with

the universe, I was also feeling a deep sense of gratitude for what I've had. I've had a terrific

life. I'm not that young. I've had a lot of it. And I had great parents, wonderful friends growing up.

I've had a good career. It's been a fairly easy run of it. And I think

the ability to have a job where you follow your own curiosity every day, there's nothing like

that. So few people in the world get to live that way. I feel incredibly grateful. And I feel

incredibly grateful for my family. And I have a wonderful wife named Dina. And she's just the

best. How do I deserve this? I don't deserve her, you know, honestly. So, you know, in neuroscience,

we often think, oh, well, there's a, you know, you have a state, you have a set point. Are you anxious?

Or are you relaxed? Are you fleeing? Or are you approaching? You know, it's like a single axis.

Well, but it isn't. You know, and I think most people understand that. But me, I didn't until

that moment really understand that I could feel profoundly grateful and profoundly angry in the

very, in the very same moment. And, you know, having cancer and getting the kind of treatments,

the chemo, the radiation, you know, it's famously deeply unpleasant. And I had all that. And it was

just as unpleasant as anyone's cancer story that you've heard, the radiation burned my esophagus.

I couldn't eat for weeks. It was months. It was painful to swallow, you know, bad stuff.

Lots of people have had to have bad stuff like this. And what I realize, you know, this is,

it's a deeply unempowering situation to be a medical patient, particularly when there's

something serious. You have, you have a limited sense of agency. Things are being done to you.

Drugs go in you that make you feel really bad. And there isn't, there isn't that much to do.

And I realized that for me, the sense of agency came from being curious,

from being a total nerd about things. And part of what it made me curious about

was my own mental processes as they related to my cancer and my cancer diagnosis.

So for example, I'm getting the chemo. And I should just say as background, I'm fortunate.

I don't have a tendency for depression. I'm a pretty upbeat guy. I don't take any credit for that.

I think I was just born lucky and raised lucky, right? But day after day of feeling bad in your

body from chemo, boy, it's hard to be positive. It really is. I could not overcome my mood,

God, really, really low. And I could tell myself, this is going to be over. It's not going to go

on forever. You won't feel this way forever. And you would think that as a rational person,

I could talk myself out of that mood, but I couldn't. You know, probably because there was,

my brain was awash in interleukin 6 and I couldn't overcome it. I felt really low. But at the same

time, I was sort of out of a move being a nerd about going like, huh, I bet these cytokines

are messing me up right now. I bet that's what's going on. And that gave me some sense of agency

in a time where otherwise, I really wouldn't have it. Another thing it really brought home to me

is this issue that we were discussing earlier about how malleable perception is and perception of time

in particular. If someone had said to me when I was healthy before I was diagnosed,

you're going to die in five years. I would have gone, oh, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, I'm 59 years

old. I should get way more than five years. I got a lot of things to do, professional things,

personal things, family things. I got a lot of things to do. No, that wouldn't be right. I'd be

very upset. But if you told me after my diagnosis of six to 18 months, oh, you get five years,

I'd be like five years. Yeah, that's pretty good. I can do a lot in five years. I can finish up in

the lab and I can do some good work and I can spend time with my family and travel and save

her life's pleasures and do all kinds of things. Five years, great. And of course, it's the same

five years. The only thing that's different is the context. But I think the thing that

really I realize the most is that I really couldn't and still can't engage with the idea

of myself being gone. So yeah, I can do practical things. I can update my will. I can write letters

for my people in my lab. So if I kick off, they've got that to take to their next job. I can do all

these nuts and bolts things. But in terms of genuinely engaging with my own demise, I really

find that as much as I try, I really can't do that. And at first I thought, well, that's just

your own lack of imagination, Linton. It's just because you're not very good at this.

But the more I thought about it, I thought, actually, no, this is a human thing. This is a

fundamental human thing. And one of the things when I look back on the 40 plus years I've been

doing neuroscience that's different is that when I was first trained, the brain was really described

as a reactive structure. Something happens in the world. It comes to your sense organs,

your eyes, your ears. It goes into your brain. It triggers some things. You think you make decisions

and then you make an action. It goes out to your muscles and that's the loop. And that's what the

brain does. And what we have known in more recent years is that actually when the brain is waiting

for something to happen, it's not just idling and spacing out. That the brain is at every moment

subconsciously trying to predict the near future. Predicting the near future is predicated on the

idea that there will be a near future. That is to say that you won't be dead and gone. That there'll

be a future for you. And so I think that my ability, which I think is actually a human,

I mean, not my ability, my failure, which I think is actually a human failure to truly engage with

my own demise is a feature. It is a side effect of the fact that the brain is always trying to predict

the future. And so that was interesting to me just as a way that my illness was revealing something

about the brain. But it also made me think a lot about the world's religions. Religion is everywhere

in the world. If you ask anthropologists, is there any society that doesn't have religious ideas

will say no. They say they don't always have the word religion. They might just say, well,

yeah, in this place, everybody knows that, you know, the world's on the back of a turtle and,

you know, this and this happened. There are these rules. They may not call it religion,

but every place in the world has religion. Not everyone is religious, but it is across cultural

universal. And most religions, absolutely every single one, but almost every single one has stories

of afterlife reincarnation in which your consciousness endures. Well, why would that be? And in many

religions, they've got a deal, right? Follow these rules in life, and then you'll be rewarded in the

afterlife. And that's a very general idea. Or punished in the afterlife. Or punished in the

afterlife, right? And, you know, in some religions, you meld with the divine. In other religions,

you're reincarnated as this or that, heaven or hell, right? There's variants, but they share

that your consciousness endures. And so why is this so popular all over the world? Well, I would

hypothesize that it is a side effect of the fact that the brain can't help but always trying to

predict the future. When we can't imagine the world without us in it, then we are forced to concoct

stories of the afterlife. Fascinating. And makes me want to ask about this

feature of time perception. My undergraduate, graduate, and postdoc advisors all sadly died

earlier, early, really, by pretty much any standard. And I was fortunate enough to be in

communication with the last two as they were going through that process. Both of them described

a heightened sense of gratitude, especially for things that previously they had not paid attention

to. So we call this noticing the little things. But that makes me conclude that something about

the knowledge of one's impending death, however far off that might be, shifts our attention,

at least temporarily, leads to this sense of slowing down a bit, because in order to shift

our attention to, quote unquote, the little things or things that we previously overlooked,

there's this sense of slowing down. And we know from basic videography, photography,

that slowing down means an increase in frame rate, that shooting at strobe frame rates gives

you the perception of strobe. Shooting at very high frame rates allows you to see things in very

slow motion. You're noticing subtle variations that normally you overlook. I'm not trying to be

overly reductionist about this process of enhanced gratitude that you describe and how it was alongside

intense anger. But have you noticed a shift in your perception of time, because you were given

initially this X number of months? And then now with the, you're still here,

fortunately. And with this kind of open-ended, well, it wasn't the prediction that was given

to you by your oncologist, but it's unclear how long you're going to be here, which is how most

of us exist. You have the sense that it's sooner rather than later, but you don't really know.

So I'm curious as to how the idea that, okay, you have 12 months more to live versus

more than 12 months, but not infinite. But of course, I know that I have,

hopefully have more than 12 months, but it's not infinite. So this idea of the finish line,

the cliff, leaving aside whatever might happen afterwards, I don't know, haven't been there.

It changes what we notice by way of changing our perception of time.

I mean, this is a profound tuning of our perception. What are your thoughts on that? And

do you notice that each sip of coffee, you probably don't notice each step across the

kitchen floor in the morning, you're probably paying attention to your lovely wife and kids and

things that day, but presumably it's dynamic. But what is your perception of time like now

with the understanding that, yes, you made it through the past, the gate that was predicted,

but what lies ahead is uncertain. Yeah. So that's really interesting. And I would say

definitely my perception of time is slower. And it seems like an age since I was diagnosed.

But I think part of that is because it's been action-packed. In other words,

since I was diagnosed, so many emotionally salient things, non-trivial things, have happened.

So many intense discussions with my wife and my friends and the people in my lab.

My wife and I have taken a lot more vacations than we normally do. So we're running all over

the world and there's a certain sense of packing it in that I think influences time perception.

But I would say actually for me personally, the gratitude isn't about the little things.

The gratitude is about the very biggest things. The gratitude is

gratitude for being a sentient being and having that blessing. The gratitude is

for being able to have a life where I can follow my own ideas and creativity. And my gratitude is for

to choke up the profound love that I've felt from my wife and my children.

You know, it's not the little stuff. It's the big stuff that I think about when I think about

gratitude. It's not noticing the sip of tea. It's the big issues. And

you know, for me, I want to delay my death as much as possible, of course.

But when I think about it, the part that makes me upset

is leaving people behind. It's not for myself. I've had a great life. I've had a lot of it.

I'm 61. I'd like to go longer, but that's a pretty good run. I've gotten to do lots of things

in those 61 years and have wonderful, loving, interconnected experiences. And so the negative

part is about what I leave behind. Certainly what you've left behind is enormous and has been

the consequence of actions long before your diagnosis, which I think is a clear lesson to

everyone. I can't speak for you, but don't wait for the diagnosis. You've mentioned the sense of

agency that you've felt by being able to pay attention to and explore your experience of,

let's call it what it is, impending death. And at the same time, as you mentioned, you've amplified

and accelerated the number of things that you've put into the world recently, writing incredible

articles about your experience of life and death. And we will, of course, link to those so people

can read them. I've read them all and they are profound and they don't just feel important,

they clearly are important. So very few people have your insight into the nervous system at

the mechanistic level, but also at this more holistic level that you've clearly displayed to

us here and in your research and in your book writing and public speaking. I think it's a

risky thing to ask somebody for advice, but I can't help myself because I think it's a

real opportunity if you had advice to give to any and all of us based on the whole experience.

All of it from go, as they say. If you're willing and feel free to pass, but if you're willing,

what is your advice? I would say the advice that is really universal is what everybody already

knows and is a bit trite, but I'll say it anyway. And that is appreciate what you got while you got

it. And this isn't any big secret and everyone knows it. I would say

for a subset of people, the way of the nerd is very empowering. I don't think that's the case for

everyone. I think for a subset of people who are deeply curious as their nature, turning that

curiosity to your own mortality and your own medical situation can be empowering and useful.

But I don't think that should be broad advice. I think that's only, for a fraction of people,

that's probably the worst thing they could do. And there's nothing wrong with that. Everybody's

a difference. Not everyone should adopt the way of the nerd, but for a fraction of people,

it's a really, really good thing to do. This is normally the portion of a conversation with a

guest where I list off the many, many things they've done and how grateful I am. And all of that is

absolutely true in the case of you being here today and the work you've done. But I think it's

self-evident how much you've not just accomplished, but how much knowledge you've put into the world

and not just scientific knowledge, but knowledge about the human experience of others and of yourself.

And so I just want to extend a giant thank you on behalf of the listeners and viewers and myself.

Thank you for coming here today. Thank you for doing what you do. And so great to still have you

here and to have this conversation. And I hope it goes longer and no matter when it ends,

you've done an enormous service to humanity. Well, thank you. That's very kind. It's been a

pure pleasure to have this discussion with you. Thanks, David. Thank you for joining me today

for this discussion with Dr. David Linden. If you're learning from and or enjoying this podcast,

please subscribe to our YouTube channel. That's a terrific zero-cost way to support us.

In addition, please subscribe to the podcast on Spotify and Apple. And on both Spotify and Apple,

you can leave us up to a five-star review. If you have questions for me or comments about the podcast

or guests that you'd like me to consider hosting on the Huberman Lab podcast, please put those in

the comment section on YouTube. I do read all the comments. Please also check out the sponsors

mentioned at the beginning and throughout today's episode. That's the best way to support this

podcast. Not on today's podcast, but on many previous episodes of the Huberman Lab podcast,

we discuss supplements. While supplements aren't necessary for everybody,

many people derive tremendous benefit from them for things like improving sleep,

hormone support, and focus. The Huberman Lab podcast has partnered with Momentus supplements.

If you'd like to access the supplements discussed on the Huberman Lab podcast,

you can go to livemomentus spelled O-U-S. So it's livemomentus.com slash Huberman.

You can also receive 20% off. Again, that's livemomentus spelled O-U-S.com slash Huberman.

If you haven't already subscribed to our neural network newsletter, our neural network newsletter

is a completely zero-cost monthly newsletter that includes summaries of podcast episodes,

as well as protocols. That is short PDFs describing, for instance, tools to improve sleep,

tools to improve neuroplasticity. We talk about deliberate cold exposure, fitness,

various aspects of mental health, again, all completely zero cost. And to sign up,

you simply go to hubermanlab.com, go over to the menu in the corner, scroll down to newsletter,

and provide your email. We do not share your email with anybody.

If you're not already following me on social media, I am Huberman Lab on all platforms.

So that's Instagram, Twitter, Threads, LinkedIn, and Facebook. And at all of those places,

I talk about science and science related tools, some of which overlaps with the content of the

Huberman Lab podcast, but much of which is distinct from the content of the Huberman Lab

podcast. Again, it's Huberman Lab on all social media platforms. Thank you once again for joining

me for my discussion with Dr. David Linden. And last but certainly not least, thank you for your

interest in science.

Machine-generated transcript that may contain inaccuracies.

In this episode, my guest is Dr. David Linden, Ph.D., professor of neuroscience at Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine and the author of many popular books about the brain. We discuss individual differences between people — focusing on differences in how people sense the world around them and the roles that chance, heredity, and life experiences (even in utero) play in determining our physical and cognitive traits. We discuss the bidirectional connection between the mind and body and how our thoughts and mental practices (e.g., meditation and breathwork) impact our health. We also discuss the link between inflammation and depression. We also discuss Dr. Linden’s terminal illness diagnosis, his mindset during chemotherapy and what his diagnosis has taught him about the mind, gratitude, time perception and life. This episode also covers sensual touch, cerebellar function, and epigenetic inheritance and ought to be of interest to all interested in neuroscience, genetics, psychology and human development. 
For the full show notes, including articles, books, and other resources, visit hubermanlab.com.
Take our survey and get 2 months of Huberman Lab Premium
Thank you to our sponsors
AG1: https://drinkag1.com/huberman
ROKA: https://roka.com/huberman
Levels: https://levels.link/huberman
InsideTracker: https://insidetracker.com/huberman
Supplements from Momentous
https://www.livemomentous.com/huberman
Timestamps
(00:00:00) David Linden
(00:03:59) Sponsors: ROKA & Levels; Huberman Lab Survey
(00:07:54) Sensory Touch & Genitals, Krause Corpuscles
(00:16:46) Sexual Experiences & Sensation
(00:19:14) Human Individuality & Variation; Senses & Odor Detection
(00:30:25) Sponsor: AG1
(00:31:22) Visual Individuality; Heat Tolerance; Early Life Experiences & Variation
(00:40:28) Auditory Variability, Perfect Pitch
(00:42:08) Heritability & Human Individuality: Cognitive & Physical Traits
(00:49:36) Heritability, Environment, Personality; Twin Studies
(01:00:12) Sponsor: InsideTracker
(01:01:19) Development, Chance; Transgenerational Epigenetic Inheritance
(01:07:37) Single Generation Epigenetic Inheritance & Stress; Autism
(01:15:52) Sleep Paralysis; Cerebellum, Prediction
(01:23:47) Nature vs. Nature, Experience; Linden Hypothesis
(01:30:37) Mind-Body Interaction; Chemical Signals
(01:39:10) Inflammation & Depression
(01:43:35) Neuroplasticity, Inflammation & Mental Disorders; Microglial Cells, Exercise 
(01:52:15) Fads & Science
(01:55:16) Mind-Body Communication; Cancer 
(02:03:28) Mind-Body, Mediation, Breathwork
(02:07:30) Atrial Fibrillation, Synovial Sarcoma, Heart 
(02:14:22) Gratitude & Anger; Chemotherapy, Curiosity & Time Perception
(02:19:58) Death, Brain & Future Prediction, Religion & Afterlife
(02:24:15) Life Advice; Time Perception & Gratitude
(02:34:35) Zero-Cost Support, YouTube Feedback, Spotify & Apple Reviews, Sponsors, Momentous, Social Media, Neural Network Newsletter
Title Card Photo Credit: Mike Blabac
Disclaimer